Pain
One of the skin senses deserves
special mention—our sense of pain. Pain usually begins with activity in the nociceptors—receptors in the skin that
have bare nerve endings and that respond to various forms of tissue damage as
well as to temperature extremes. These receptors come in two types: A-delta fibers allow rapid transmission
of information and are responsible for the pain you experience when you’re
first injured.
The C fibers, in contrast, are unmyelinated and therefore slower in
their transmission; they’re the source of the dull ache that remains long after
the injury occurs.
Activity in the nociceptors is
aversive—obviously, we’d prefer to avoid pain! But pain plays a crucial role
for us—as evident in the rare individuals who have medical conditions making
them insensitive to pain (Melzack & Wall, 1982). These individuals are
constantly at risk: They can’t feel the pain of resting a hand on a hot stove,
so they’re in danger of burning themselves. They can’t feel the irritation of
an object caught in the throat, so they don’t cough—creating a danger of
choking. They don’t notice if they bite their own tongues while chewing food.
These (and many other) examples remind us that pain is useful—helping us to avoid a wide range of hazards. Without this
“alert system,” ourlikelihood of serious injury would be a lot greater and our
life expectancies much shorter.
Let’s be clear, though, that the
experience of pain depends on far more than the noci-ceptors themselves because
there are circumstances in which people appear to feel no pain despite activity
in these receptors. The classic examples come from soldiers who appear not to
notice their wounds until the battle ends, or athletes who don’t feel a
sprained ankle, or even a broken rib, until the excitement of the game
subsides.
How should we think about these
observations? Part of the answer involves substances, produced by our bodies, that
function as internal medications. These substances—which we first met—are
chemically quite similar to drugs like morphine or codeine, and they interfere
with the neurotransmission involved in pain sensation. These natural internal
painkillers, called endorphins, help us in many cir-cumstances to continue
functioning despite a seemingly painful injury.
Pain sensations can also be
blocked via a different mechanism: According to the gatecontrol theory, pain sensations must pass through a neural
“gate” to reach the brainand can be blocked at that gate by neurons that
inhibit signals from the nociceptors, so that these signals are never
transmitted to the brain. The gate neurons that provide this inhibition can be
triggered in several ways, including counterirritation.
This term refers to the phenomenon in which painful sensations from one part of
the body trigger the gate neurons and thus decrease the sensitivity to pain
elsewhere in the body (e.g., Motohashi & Umino, 2001).
We also know that the experience of
pain is influenced by someone’s beliefs and emotions. However, these cognitive
factors may be affecting the emotion that accom-panies pain rather than the
sensation of pain itself (Melzack & Casey, 1968). These two aspects of
pain—the sensation and the emotion—can be distinguished subjec-tively. They can
also be distinguished functionally—so that hypnosis (for example) seems to
alter the emotional response to pain without decreasing the actual intensity of
the sensation. These two aspects of pain can also be distinguished by brain
activity—the sensation corresponds mostly to activation in specific areas of
the somatosensory cortex, and the emotion corresponds more closely to activity
in the anterior cingulate cortex.
Related Topics
Privacy Policy, Terms and Conditions, DMCA Policy and Compliant
Copyright © 2018-2023 BrainKart.com; All Rights Reserved. Developed by Therithal info, Chennai.