Judgment and
Reasoning: An Overview
There are both parallels and
contrasts between judgment and reasoning. In both domains, we find uneven
performance—sometimes people are capable of wonderfully high-quality thinking,
and sometimes they make outrageous errors in their judging or reasoning. We
also find, in both judgment and reasoning, that various factors or cues within
a problem can trigger better quality thinking (System 2)—so that the way
someone thinks depends heav-ily on what they’re thinking about. Thus, when
thinking about a Sunday football game, peo-ple are alert to the role of chance
and wary of drawing conclusions from a single game. The same people, in
thinking about a job interview, might not realize the sample of information is
small and so might overinterpret the evidence. Likewise, people’s performance
in the selection task is fine if the problem contains cues suggesting a
possibility of cheating or a need for permission; the same people perform
miserably without these cues.
Judgment and reasoning differ,
though, in how they proceed in the absence of these triggers. In making
judgments, we often rely on System 1 thinking—a set of strategies that
generally lead us to sensible conclusions and that are quick and efficient. But
there’s no obvious parallel to these strategies in many reasoning tasks—for
example, when we’re trying to evaluate an if-then sentence (like the one in the
selection task). This situation is reflected in the fact that people don’t make
occasional errors with logic
problems—instead, we’ve mentioned error rates of 80 and 90%! It’s fortunate,
therefore, that our daily experience unfolds in a context in which the triggers
we need, leading us into better quality reasoning, are often in place.
One last parallel between
judgment and reasoning is important and quite encouraging: In both domains,
training helps. We’ve mentioned that courses in statistics, and training in the
interpretation of data, seem to improve people’s judgment—perhaps by making
them more sensitive to the need to gather an ade-quate sample of evidence and
by making them more cautious about samples that may be biased. Education also
improves people’s ability to reason well—and, again, the education seems to
help by making people more alert to cues that might trigger decent
reasoning—cues that help people to think about issues (for example) of
per-mission or obligation (Lehman & Nisbett, 1990). Thus, we can offer the
optimistic conclusion that, with the appropriate training , people can learn to
think more care-fully and accurately than they ordinarily do.
Related Topics
Privacy Policy, Terms and Conditions, DMCA Policy and Compliant
Copyright © 2018-2023 BrainKart.com; All Rights Reserved. Developed by Therithal info, Chennai.