The
Availability Heuristic
In almost all cases, we want our
conclusions to rest not just on one observation, but on patterns of observations. Is last-minute cramming an effective way
to prepare for exams?Does your car start more easily if you pump the gas while
turning the key? Do you get sick less often if you take vitamin tablets? In
each case, you could reach a conclusion based on just one experience (one exam,
or one flu season), but that’s a risky strategy because that experience might
have been a fluke of some sort, or in some way atypical. Thus, what you really
want is a summary of multiple
experiences, so that you can draw conclusions only if there’s a consistent
pattern in the evidence.
Generally, this summary of
multiple experiences will require a comparison among frequency estimates—assessments
of how often you’ve encountered a particular event orobject. How often have you
crammed for an exam and done well? How often have you crammed and done poorly?
How many people do you know who take vitamins and still get sick? How many stay
healthy?
In this way, frequency estimates
are central for judgment, but there’s an obvious problem here: Most people
don’t keep ledgers recording the events of their lives, and so they have no
objective record of what happened each time they started their car or how many
of their friends take vitamins. What do people do, then, when they need
frequency estimates? They rely on a simple strategy: They try to think of
spe-cific cases relevant to their judgment—exams that went well after cramming,
or frustrating mornings when the car just wouldn’t start. If these examples
come easily to mind, people conclude that the circumstance is a common one; if
the examples come to mind slowly or only with great effort, people conclude
that the circumstance is rare.
This strategy is referred to as
the availability heuristic, because
the judgment uses availability (i.e.,
how easily the cases come to mind) as the basis for assessing frequency (how common the cases
actually are in the world). For many frequency estimates this strategy works
well, because objects or events that are broadly frequent in the world are
likely to be frequent in our personal experience and are therefore well
represented in our memories. On this basis, “easily available from memory” is
often a good indicator of “frequent in the world.”
But there are surely
circumstances in which this strategy is misleading. In one study, participants
were asked this question: “Considering all the words in the lan-guage, does R occur more frequently in the first
position of the word (rose, robot,rocket)
or in the third position (care, strive,
tarp)?” Over two-thirds of the participantssaid that R is more common the first position—but actually the reverse is
true, by a wide margin.
What caused this error?
Participants made this judgment by trying to think of words in which R is the first letter, and these came
easily to mind. They next tried to think of words in which it’s the third
letter, and these came to mind only with some effort. They then interpreted
this difference in ease of retrieval (i.e., the difference in availability) as if
it reflected a difference in frequency—and so drew the wrong conclusion. As it
turns out, the difference in retrieval merely shows that our mental dictionary,
roughly like a printed one, is organized according to the starting sound of
each word. This arrange-ment makes it easy to search memory using a word’s
“starting letter” as the cue; a search based on a word’s third letter is more
difficult. In this fashion, the organization of memory creates a bias in what’s
easily available; this bias, in turn, leads to an error in frequency judgment
(Tversky & Kahneman, 1973).
In this task, it seems sensible
for people to use a shortcut (the heuristic) rather than some more laborious
strategy, such as counting through the pages in a dictionary. The latter
strategy would guarantee the right answer but would surely be far more work than
the problem is worth. In addition, the error in this case is harmless—nothing
hinges on these assessments of spelling patterns. The problem, though, is that
people rely on the same shortcut—using availability to assess frequency—in
cases that are more con-sequential. For example, many friendships break up
because of concerns over fairness: “Why am I always the one who does the
dishes?” Or “Why is it that you’re usually the one who starts our fights, but
I’m always the one who reaches out afterward?” These questions hinge on
frequency estimates—and use of the availability heuristic routinely leads to
errors in these estimates (M. Ross & Siccoly, 1979). As a result, this
judgment heuristic may leave us with a distorted perception of some social
relations—in a way that can undermine some friendships!
As a different example, what are
the chances that the stock market will go up tomor-row or that a certain
psychiatric patient will commit suicide? The stockbrokers and psy-chiatrists
who make these judgments regularly base their decisions on an estimate of
probabilities. In the past, has the market generally gone up after a
performance like today’s? In the past, have patients with these symptoms
generally been dangerous to themselves? These estimates, too, are likely to be
based on the availability heuristic. Thus, for example, the psychiatrist’s
judgment may be poor if he vividly remembers a particular patient who
repeatedly threatened suicide but never harmed himself. This easily available
recollection may bias the psychiatrist’s frequency judgment, leading to
inadequate precautions in the present case.
Related Topics
Privacy Policy, Terms and Conditions, DMCA Policy and Compliant
Copyright © 2018-2023 BrainKart.com; All Rights Reserved. Developed by Therithal info, Chennai.