Dissociative Disorders
There
is no question that many disorders—and, indeed, many life experiences— involve
considerable pain or distress. To protect themselves against this discomfort,
people engage in a variety of defensive strategies. One of these strategies,
which we have mentioned in other contexts, is dissociation—a term that covers the various ways in which people
try to distance themselves psychologically from ongoing events. They establish
this distance by saying things like “This can’t be happening” or “This is all a
dream” or “This isn’t happening to me.” With these defenses in place, people
may, during a calamity, even experience themselves as eerily calm, floating
outside their own bodies as they watch themselves react.
These
adjustments are often adaptive, a way of coping with horrific events. But such
adjustments can go too far and, in their extreme form, are the defining feature
of the dissociative disorders. In some cases, for example, a person develops dissociativeamnesia and is suddenly
unable to remember some period of his life, or even hisentire past, including
his own identity. Such episodes usually last less than 1 week (Kihlstrom &
Schacter, 2000; Maldonado, Butler, & Spiegel, 2002). In other cases, the
dissociation produces dissociative fugue,
in which the person wanders away from home and then, days or even months later,
suddenly realizes that she is in a strange place, does not know how she got
there, and has total amnesia for the entire period.
Far
more controversial is the diagnosis of dissociative
identity disorder (DID), a condition formerly known as multiple personality
disorder. Here the dissociation is said to be so massive that it results in two
or more distinct personalities, each with its own style, habits, and beliefs.
The personalities often differ in their interests, their age, their sexual
orientation, and their morals.
In
one case, a patient named Julie mentioned, during therapy, that she wanted to
introduce someone to the therapist. She closed her eyes, frowned, and then
opened her eyes slowly, asserting that she was Jerrie, not Julie. Roughly 1
hour later, in the same way, she introduced Jenny, a third personality in the
same body. These three women were different in many ways: Julie and Jenny were
heterosexuals; Jerrie was a lesbian. Jenny was frightened and insecure; Julie
was a sophisticated and caring mother. Jerrie was accomplished and proficient
in the business world and hated the fact that Julie was a heavy smoker (Davis
& Osherson, 1977).
Until
several decades ago, cases like this were rare, with fewer than 200 reported
before 1975. Now cases number in the thousands; the large majority are women
(Kluft, 1987; C. A. Ross, 1999). The number of personalities reported in each
case has also increased dramatically: A review of the early cases indicated
that most people with DID had only 2 or 3 separate identities (Taylor &
Martin, 1944); a more recent survey indicates that the average number of
identities reported by patients is 15, with some cases reporting far more than
this (C. A. Ross, 1997).
These
massive increases are one of the reasons this disorder has been a focus of so
much debate, with some critics arguing that the flood of diagnoses may reflect
a fad among therapists who inadvertently lead suggestible patients to develop
the signs and
symptoms
of the syndrome (Lalonde, Hudson, Gigante, & Pope, 2001; Lilienfeld et al.,
1999; Spanos, 1996; for a very different view, see Lewis, Yeager, Swica,
Pincus, & Lewis, 1997). Contributing to this trend, the critics claim, are
the books and movies about DID, including Sybil
and The Three Faces of Eve (Figure
16.35).
What
should we make of this debate? It seems likely that at least some cases of DID
are in fact faked or inadvertently created by therapists. But it also seems
likely that many cases are genuine, and debate continues about the nature of
this disorder, its causes, and the best way to treat it (Lilienfeld et al.,
1999; C. A. Ross, 1999).
Stepping
away from the controversy over DID, and returning to the broader category of
dissociative disorders, we can ask: What factors or events lead to these
disorders? Here, too, some investigators propose a genetic basis, but research
has yielded mixed results (Jang, Paris, Zweig-Frank, & Livesley, 1998;
Waller & Ross, 1997). One thing we do know is that people naturally seem to
differ in how much, and under what circumstances, they dissociate. Some of the
relevant evidence comes from a measure known as the Dissociative Experiences Scale,
which asks people directly how often they have experienced various perceptions
or behaviors characteristic of dissociation (Carlson & Putnam, 1993). Other
evidence comes from work on hypnosis, which some investigators regard as a form
of guided dissociation. People differ enormously in how readily or how deeply
they can be hypnotized (Ganaway, 1989), and this opens an interesting
possibility: Perhaps some people were, as children, particularly adept at
self-hypnosis and learned to use this skill as a defense—putting themselves
into a self-induced hypnotic trance to escape from the stresses (or perhaps
traumas) in their lives (Bliss, 1980). This skill may then create a readiness
to dissociate in everyday life, per-haps predisposing these individuals to develop
a dissociative disorder.
Thus,
a skill in (or habit of ) dissociation may create the diathesis for a
dissociative dis-order. To precipitate the full-fledged disorder, however,
there also has to be some unusual stress, and, in fact, most cases of dissociative
amnesia occur after the same kinds of cata-clysmic events that lead to
post-traumatic stress disorder. The same holds for dissociative fugues, which
can also develop suddenly after personal misfortunes or financial pressures
(Andreasen & Black, 1996). The same may be true as well for dissociative
identity disor-der: In a large percentage of DID case histories, there are
terrifying stories of brutal phys-ical or sexual abuse in childhood, often
including incest (Putnam, Guroff, Silberman, Barban, & Post, 1986).
However, these reports—like the diagnosis of DID itself—have been
controversial, inasmuch as they often rely on the person’s recall of the abuse, and, as we discussed, there has been debate
over how to interpret this recall.
Related Topics
Privacy Policy, Terms and Conditions, DMCA Policy and Compliant
Copyright © 2018-2023 BrainKart.com; All Rights Reserved. Developed by Therithal info, Chennai.