Home | | Psychology | Psychology Language: The Meanings of Sentences

Chapter: Psychology: Language

Psychology Language: The Meanings of Sentences

Psychology Language: The Meanings of Sentences
COMPLEX SENTENCE MEANINGS, AMBIGUITY IN WORDS AND SENTENCES.

The Meanings of Sentences

Sentences have meanings too, over and above the meanings of the words they contain. This is obvious from the fact that two sentences can be composed of all and only the same words and yet be meaningfully distinct. For example, The giraffe bit the zebra and The zebra bit the giraffe describe different events, a meaning difference of some impor-tance, at least to the zebra and the giraffe.

The typical sentence introduces some topic (the subject of the sentence) and then makes some comment, or offerunderstand a sentence, then, is to determine which actors are portraying the various roles and what the plot (the action) is; that is, to decide on the basis of sentence structure who did what to whom.

For the very simplest sentences of a language, their grammatical structure links up rather directly to the semantics of who-did-what-to-whom (M. C. Baker, 2001; Grimshaw, 1990; Jackendoff, 2002). To get a feel for how this system works, see Figure 10.15A, which shows the phrase structure of the zebra sentence. We can “read” the semantic roles off of this syntactic tree by attending to its geometry, as shown in Figure 10.15B. The doer of the action is the noun phrase that branches directly from the root of the tree (a position known as “the sentence subject,” namely, the zebra). The “done-to” is the noun phrase that branches off of the verb phrase (a position known as “the direct object,” namely, the giraffe). So different noun phrases in the syntactic struc-ture have different semantic roles. More complex sentences of the language will encode yet further semantic roles and relations, for example, The nanny (do-er) feeds (act) somesoup (done-to, or thing affected) to the baby (recipient) with a spoon (instrument).


Summarizing, the position of the words and phrases in this simple sentence of English is providing the semantic role information to the listener. Serialization is of cen-tral importance in this regard in English and many other languages. However, there is another type of information, used in the English pronoun system, that can signal the semantic roles without regard to serial position. For instance, the sentences He hadalways admired her intelligence andHer intelligence is what he had always admired mean thesame thing. It is the particular form of a pronoun (he versus him, she versus her) that assigns the semantic roles in this case. Many languages use something analogous to our pronoun system as the primary means for signaling semantic roles, for nouns as well as pronouns. They do this with case markers, which usually occur as function morphemes. When case markers occur regularly in a language, the word order itself can be much more flexible than it is in English. In Finnish, for example, the words meaning zebra and giraffe change their serial order in Seepra puri kirahvia and Kirahvia puri seepra, but in either order it is the giraffe (kirahvia) who needs the medical attention. It is the suffix -a (rather than -ia) that in both sentences identifies the zebra as the aggressor in this battle.

COMPLEX SENTENCE MEANINGS

In very simple sentences, as we have seen, the phrase structure straightforwardly reflects the propositional meaning of who-did-what-to-whom. But the sentences we encounter in everyday conversation are usually much more complicated than these first examples. Many factors contribute to complexity. Sometimes we reorder the phrases so as to emphasize aspects of the scene other than the doer (It was the giraffe who got bittenby the zebra). Sometimes we wish to question (Who bit the giraffe?) or command (Bite that giraffe!) rather than merely comment on the passing scene. And often we wish tos some information, about that topic (the predicate). Thus, when we say, The giraffe bit the zebra, we introduce the giraffe as the topic, and then we propose or predicate of the giraffe that it bit the zebra. Accordingly, sentence meanings are often called propositions. In effect, a simple sentence describes a minia-ture drama in which the verb is the action and the nouns are the performers, each playing a different semantic role. In The zebra bit the giraffe, the zebra plays the role of doer or agent who causes or instigates the action, the giraffe is the done-to—the thing affected by the action, and biting is the action itself. The job of a listener who wants to express our attitudes (beliefs, hopes, and so forth) toward certain events (I was delightedto hear that the zebra bit the giraffe), or to relate one proposition to another, and so willutter two or more of them in the same sentence (The zebrathat arrived from Kenyabitthe giraffe). All this added complexity of meaning is mirrored by corresponding com-plexities in the sentence structures themselves.

Most ordinary language users fluently utter, write, and understand many sentences 50, 100, or more morphemes in length. The example below is from a blogger writing about ordering beauty supplies on line—so we can hardly protest that it was the unusual creation of some linguistic Einstein or literary giant:

However, I didn’t know how to order it and came across your site where you found free trial supplies which is great because i dont want to pay for something i didnt know worked

This author has managed to cram nine verbs in four different tenses into an infor-mal but intricate grammatical narrative. How did he or she manage to do it? Surely not by memorizing superficial recipes like “an English sentence can end with two verbs in a row” but rather by nonconsciously appreciating combinatorial regularities of enor-mous generality and power that build up sentence units like acrobats’ pyramids. The complex sentences are constructed by reusing the same smallish set of syntax rules that formed the simple sentences, but tying them all together using function morphemes for the nails and glue (Chomsky, 1959, 1981a; Z. Harris, 1951; Joshi, 2002).

AMBIGUITY IN WORDS AND SENTENCES

Not only do sentences often become complex. Very often a sentence can be interpreted more than one way: It is ambiguous. Sometimes, the ambiguity depends only on one word having two or more meanings (that is, having more than one “entry” in our men-tal dictionaries); examples can be seen in such newspaper headlines as Children’s StoolsUseful for Garden Work; Red Tape Holds up Bridge; Prostitutes Appeal to Pope. In many cases,though, the ambiguity depends on structure: in particular, on alternate ways that words are grouped together in the syntactic tree (Police Are Ordered to Stop Drinking on Campus;Oil Given Free to Customers in Glass Bottles). There is a big difference, after all, betweenpolice who [stop drinking] [on campus] and those who [stop] [drinking on campus].*

Such structural ambiguity is pervasive in everyday speech and writing, even with short and apparently simple sentences. For instance, Smoking cigarettes can be dangerous could be a warning that to inhale smoke could harm your health, or a warning that if you leave your butt smoldering in an ashtray, your house might burn down.

Study Material, Lecturing Notes, Assignment, Reference, Wiki description explanation, brief detail
Psychology: Language : Psychology Language: The Meanings of Sentences |


Privacy Policy, Terms and Conditions, DMCA Policy and Compliant

Copyright © 2018-2024 BrainKart.com; All Rights Reserved. Developed by Therithal info, Chennai.