MOTIVES BEYOND DRIVES
We
can understand the motives we have considered so far principally in terms of
drive-reduction. In each case, the motive involves some internal
tension—feeling cold or hot, feeling hungry, feeling threatened, or feeling
sexual needs—and the motivated behaviors help us reduce (or escape) the
tension. But surely we are motivated not only to avoid negative experiences, but also to seek positive experiences. Consider the satisfac-tion of solving a
difficult problem, the exhilaration of riding a roller coaster or dancing, or
the ecstasy of fulfilled love. Plainly we value these kinds of experiences for
their own sake. Likewise, people will get out of bed and walk out into the cold
simply to enjoy a beautiful sunrise. People will also spend money to see a play
or attend a rock concert. All of these activities are difficult to understand
in terms of drive-reduction.
In
this section, we broaden our consideration of motivational states, and consider
motives that go beyond tension-reduction, and are about more than survival.
Theorists have described many, many such motives (including motives that lead
people to seek affil-iation, achievement, autonomy, control, competence, power,
self-esteem, and differentia-tion, among others), and here we will consider two
major motives that govern our daily activities, namely, our motive to belong to
groups and our motive to achieve.
We
all want to belong, to have friends, and to fit in (Leary & Cox, 2008).
Indeed, we go to great lengths to seek out and maintain friendships, and we are
upset when our rela-tionships are severed. As we will see, much of our mental
lives are spent considering others’ thoughts and feelings toward us.
Abraham
Maslow (1968, 1996) was one of the early theorists who insisted that to understand
what is truly human, psychologists must consider all our motives, and he
recognized in particular the motive to belong as a powerful force in human
behavior (Figure 12.23). Most psychologists agree. For example, Baumeister and
Leary (1995) put it clearly when they said: “The need to belong is a powerful,
fundamen-tal, and extremely pervasive motivation”. Thus, people may go without
food to make themselves more attractive to others, and they may risk life and
limb to impress others around them.
At least part of the need to belong can be understood as yet another form of drive-reduction: We do not want to be alone, or rejected, and so we take steps to avoid these experiences (Schultheiss, 2008). But it is crucial that the motive to belong also has a positive “approach-orientation,” as we seek out the many benefits of being with others. This latter, more positive aspect of affiliation is evident, for example, in the fact that social interaction is a powerful predictor of positive emotion (Watson & Clark, 1994). In addition, a lack of meaningful social interaction predicts a range of negative psychological and physical health outcomes.
Indeed,
social contact provides many benefits. For example, other people can provide tangible support, and, in fact, few of
us would get by without practical help from manydifferent people—whether it’s
borrowing money to do laundry or getting help proof-reading an assignment.
Others also provide emotional support.
Sometimes this sup-port is direct, as when a friend consoles us when we are sad
or distracts us when we are angry; sometimes it is indirect, taking the form of
the many emotional connections to others that give our lives texture and
meaning. A third reason we seek out others is that how we feel about ourselves
is powerfully shaped by how others view us. Indeed, some researchers have
argued that self-esteem is an internal readout of how one is faring socially
(Leary, Tambor, Terdal, & Downs, 1995).
Imagine
that all of your social needs and all of your drive-based needs mentioned were
satisfied, and that you were warm, comfortable, safe, well-fed, and sur-rounded
by people who love you. That probably sounds pretty wonderful indeed—but not
perfect. What’s missing? For most of us, even this peaceful state of affairs
wouldn’t be entirely fulfilling, because it wouldn’t satisfy the motive to
create, accomplish, and achieve.
Like
the motive to belong, the motive to achieve has a dual aspect—part avoidance,
part approach. Half a century ago, McClelland and colleagues suggested that
achieve-ment-related behavior can arise either from a fear of failure
(avoidance) or from a desire for success (approach) (D. C. McClelland,
Atkinson, Clark, & Lowell, 1953). One might think that these two aspects of
the achievement motive would be tightly correlated, but they are in fact
independent, and they operate differently. It is individuals with a desire for
success—rather than a fear of failure—who seek out challenges and excel when
the going gets difficult (D. C. McClelland, 1989).
Achievement
motivation matters, and in studies of high-school and college students,
Duckworth and Seligman (2005) found that achievement motivation was a powerful
predictor of school performance. A similar picture emerges from studies of
scholars, artists, and athletes who were at the top of their respective fields
(B. S. Bloom, 1995). Talent matters, but achievement motivation also matters a
great deal (Figure 12.24).
What
factors give rise to a fear of failure or a desire for success (Koestner &
McClelland, 1990)? Early researchers focused on parenting style, and found some
evidence that caregivers who punish failure but take success for granted can
instill a fear of failure. By contrast, caregivers can promote a more positive
desire for success by rewarding achievement but not punishing failure. More
recent research has focused on cognitive factors, such as whether a child
adopts a mastery orientation, which
is characterized by a focus on learning and improving, or a performance ori-entation, characterized
by a focus on performing well in front of others in order tolook smart, or on
avoiding failure to keep from looking stupid (Dweck, 1999; 2006). Two decades
of research suggest that a mastery orientation is associated with high levels
of interest and a deep engagement with the material. When individuals with a
mastery orientation encounter adversity (such as a bad grade), they are likely
to increase their effort and seek out ways of benefiting from the experience.
By contrast, when individuals with a performance orientation get negative
feedback, they are more likely to withdraw effort and shift their focus
elsewhere (Senko, Durik, & Harackiewicz, 2008).
Related Topics
Privacy Policy, Terms and Conditions, DMCA Policy and Compliant
Copyright © 2018-2023 BrainKart.com; All Rights Reserved. Developed by Therithal info, Chennai.