Cognitive Development in Adolescence
As
we have seen, infants and children make astonishing intellectual strides
between birth and preadolescence, so that 10- and 11-year-olds are quite
sophisticated in their cognitive capacities. Even so, preadolescents’ cognitive
abilities are limited in an impor-tant way. They have gained skill in a variety
of mental operations, but they seem to apply these operations only to relations
between concrete events; this is why Piaget referred to this stage of thinking
as relying on concrete operations.
What
did Piaget mean by “concrete” thinking? Typical 8- and 9-year-olds can easily
see that 4 is an even number and 4 + 1 is odd. Similarly, they understand that
6 is even, while 6 + 1 is odd, and likewise for 8 and 8 + 1. But the same
children fail to see the inevitability of this pattern; they fail to see that
the addition of 1 to any even number must always produce a number that is odd.
According to Piaget, children are not able to comprehend this abstract and
formal relationship until about age 11 or 12, when they enter the formal operational period.
Piaget
argued that when children enter this final stage, their ability to reason and
solve problems takes a large step forward. They are now able to think about the
pos-sible as well as the real. This change is evident in an early study by Ward
and Overton (1990), who asked children of various ages to perform tasks that
required simple steps of logical reasoning. Roughly 15% of the 4th graders they
tested were able to master the tasks, in contrast to 25% of the 6th graders and
roughly 50% of the 8th graders. By the 12th grade, approximately 80% of the
children showed evidence of logical reasoning.
Children
apply these new capacities in their schoolwork by thinking about scientific
hypothesis testing or mathematical proofs in a way they could not before. They
also apply these capacities to their own lives. They imagine new possibilities
in their social relations, in politics, or in religion and may start to
challenge beliefs and conventions that had seemed beyond question just a few
years earlier.
There
is room for debate, however, about whether we should think of these changes in
the terms Piaget described. Some theorists believe that adolescent cognition is
not funda-mentally different from the thinking of middle childhood (e.g.,
Siegler, 1998). What, then, produces the advances in reasoning just described?
The answer may lie in the fact that older children have acquired a set of more
efficient strategies, and also have markedly greater memory capacity, compared
to younger children. This memory capacity (among its other benefits) allows
adolescents to relate different aspects of a task to one another in ways they
could not at an earlier age, and this is why their intellectual performance
takes a large step forward.
No
matter how we conceptualize these changes, though, one other point is crucial.
Adolescents’ thinking is highly variable, using sophisticated logic in some
cases, but relying on much more concrete strategies in other settings. This is
evident in the laboratory, and also in adolescents’ day-to-day thinking (when,
for example, they are exquisitely thoughtful in their challenges to political
institutions but then remarkably short-sighted when thinking about the
consequences of drinking and driving).
Similar
variability is crucial when we compare adolescents (or adults) in different
cultures. Adults in many parts of the world fail Piaget’s tests of formal
operations, invit-ing the notion that only a minority of the world’s people
achieve formal thinking. Other studies, however, paint a different portrait—and
suggest that cultural differences tell us more about when and where people use
logical thinking than about whether people can
use logical thinking.
Related Topics
Privacy Policy, Terms and Conditions, DMCA Policy and Compliant
Copyright © 2018-2023 BrainKart.com; All Rights Reserved. Developed by Therithal info, Chennai.