Home | | Psychology | Attachment - Socioemotional Development in Infancy and Childhood

Chapter: Psychology: Development

Attachment - Socioemotional Development in Infancy and Childhood

As children start learning about the social world—developing expectations for how others will behave and learning to “read” the signals that others provide.

ATTACHMENT

 

As children start learning about the social world—developing expectations for how others will behave and learning to “read” the signals that others provide—they are also forming their first social relationship, with their primary caregiver, usually the mother. This is evident in the fact that, when they are between 6 and 8 months old, infants begin to show a pattern known as separation anxiety, in which they become visibly (and sometimes loudly) upset when their caregiver leaves the room. This is a powerful indication that the infant has formed an attachment to the caregiver—a strong, enduring, emotional bond.

 

This bond seems to grow out of the psychological comfort the mother provides the infant. Evidence for this claim comes from many sources, including a series of classic studies by Harry Harlow (1905–1981). Harlow (e.g., 1958) raised rhesus monkeys with-out their mothers; each rhesus infant lived alone in a cage that contained two station-ary figures, one built of wire and the other of soft terry cloth. The wire figure was equipped with a nipple that yielded milk, but the terry-cloth model had none. Which figure would the monkey infants prefer—the one that provided nutrition, or the one that provided (what Harlow called) “contact comfort”? In fact, the infants spent much




more time on the terry-cloth “mother” than on the wire figure—especially when they were frightened. When approached by a clanking mechanical toy, they invariably rushed to the terry-cloth mother and clung tightly (Figure 14.20).

 

Is comfort (and not nutrition) also the key for human attachment? British psychia-trist John Bowlby argued forcefully that it is. In Bowlby’s view, children become attached to a caregiver because this adult provides a secure base for the child, a rela-tionship (and, for the young infant, a place) in which the child feels safe and protected (Figure 14.21). The child uses the secure base as a haven in times of threat, and, accord-ing to Bowlby, this provides the child with the sense of safety that allows her to explore the environment. The child is able to venture into the unknown, and explore, and learn, because she knows that, if the going gets rough, she can always return to the secure base (Waters & Cummings, 2000).

 

Bowlby argued that this tendency to form attachments, and to rely on the psycho-logical comfort of a secure base, is rooted in our evolutionary past, and this is why we can easily find parallels to human attachment in other species. For example, consider the process of imprinting, a kind of learning that, in many species, provides the basis for an infant’s attachment to its mother (e.g., Lorenz, 1935). To take a specific case, as soon as a duckling can walk (about 12 hours after hatching), it will approach and fol-low virtually any moving stimulus. If the duckling follows this moving object for about 10 minutes, an attachment is formed, and the bird has now imprinted on this object. From this point forward, the duckling will continue to follow the attachment object, show distress if separated from it, and run to it in circumstances involving threat.

 

This simple kind of learning is usually quite effective, since the first moving stimu-lus a duckling sees is usually its mother. But imprinting can occur to other objects, by accident or through an experimenter’s manipulations. In some studies, ducklings have


been exposed to a moving toy duck on wheels, or to a rectangle sliding back and forth behind a glass window, or even to the researcher’s booted legs (Figure 14.22). In each case, the ducklings follow these objects as if following their mother, uttering plaintive distress calls whenever the attachment object is not nearby (E. H. Hess, 1959, 1973).

 

Do human infants show a similar pattern of imprinting? Some studies have compared the attachment of infants who had after-birth contact with their mothers to those who did not (usually due to medical complications in either mother or baby); the studies found little difference between the groups, and hence provided no evidence for a specific period during which human newborns form attachments (Eyer, 1992). Similarly, infants who must spend their initial days with a nurse or in an incubator do not form a lasting attachment to either. Clearly, then, the pattern of imprint-to-object-of-first-exposure, common in many species, is not found in humans.

 

Even with these differences between humans and other animals, the fact remains that human infants form a strong attachment to a caregiver, usually the mother— presumably because young children in most cultures spend the vast majority of their time with their mothers (M. E. Lamb, 1987, 1997). What role does this leave for the father? Infants do develop strong attachments to both parental figures (Pipp, Easterbrooks, & Brown, 1993), but mothers and fathers typically behave differently with their infant children, and there are corresponding differences in attachment. Fathers tend to be more physical and vigorous in their play, bouncing their children or tossing them in the air. In contrast, mothers generally play more quietly with their chil-dren, telling them stories or reciting nursery rhymes and providing hugs and caresses rather than tumbles and bounces. The children act accordingly, tending to run to the mother for care and comfort but to the father for active play (Figure 14.23; Clarke-Stewart, 1978; M. E. Lamb, 1997; Parke, 1981).


 

Study Material, Lecturing Notes, Assignment, Reference, Wiki description explanation, brief detail
Psychology: Development : Attachment - Socioemotional Development in Infancy and Childhood |

Related Topics

Psychology: Development


Privacy Policy, Terms and Conditions, DMCA Policy and Compliant

Copyright © 2018-2024 BrainKart.com; All Rights Reserved. Developed by Therithal info, Chennai.