Person Perception
When
we make causal attributions, we go beyond the information available to our
senses in order to make sense of a particular action—why someone smiled, or did
well on a test, or didn’t help more after an accident. Th13 role of social
cognition— and our tendency to supplement what we perceive—is just as important
when we try to make sense of another person—that is, whenever we ask ourselves,
“What kind of person is she?”
Imagine
that a friend says her sister Marie is especially outgoing. Or say that you see Marie at several parties, and each time she’s
the center of attention. In either case, you’re likely to draw some inferences
from what you hear about Marie or how you see her behaving—but in the process,
you may lose track of which bits of information are based directly on what
you’ve heard or seen and which bits are merely your interpretation. Thus, if
you hear that Marie is outgoing, you may also remember (falsely) that your
friend said she loves crowds, even though that was never mentioned (N. Cantor
& Mischel, 1979; Dweck, Chiu, & Hong, 1995).
In
this way, social cognition works much like other kinds of cognition. Our
knowl-edge of the world is a blend of our own observations and the inferences
we have made using our schematic knowledge—knowledge
about the world in general. In the social domain, these schemas are called implicit theories of personality
(Bruner & Tagiuri, 1954; D. J. Schneider, 1973), and we use them to make
inferences about what a person is really like and how she is likely to behave
in the future. We all hold these informal the-ories, and they bring together a
cluster of beliefs, linking one trait (such as extraversion) to others (such as
loving crowds) but also linking these traits to specific behaviors—so that we have
expectations for what sort of husband the extravert will be, what sorts of
sports he is likely to enjoy, and more.
These implicit theories are another point on which cultures differ (Church et al., 2006). People in individualistic cultures tend to understand the self as being stable across time and situations and so are more likely to go beyond the information given and make global judgments about others’ personalities. People in collectivistic cul-tures, where the self is understood as changing according to relationships and situa-tions, tend to view personality as malleable. They therefore tend to make more cautious and more limited generalizations about other people’s personalities (Hong, Chiu, & Dweck, 1997).
Implicit
theories of personality—like schemas in any other domain—are enormously
helpful. Thanks to these schemas, we do not have to scrutinize every aspect of
every situation we encounter, because we can fill in any information we are missing
by drawing on our schematic knowledge. Likewise, if our current information
about a person is lim-ited (perhaps because we only met her briefly), we can
still make reasonable assumptions about her based on what we know about people
in general. Though these steps make our social perception quick and efficient
and generally lead us to valid inferences, they do leave us vulnerable to
error, so that our perception (or recollection) of another person some-times
ends up more in line with our preconceptions than with the facts. In many cases
this is a small price to pay for the benefits of using schemas, and for that
matter, the errors pro-duced are often easily corrected. But schemas can also
lead to more serious errors.
The
hazard of schematic thinking is particularly clear in the case of social stereo-types, schemas about the
characteristics of whole groups that lead us to talk aboutGreeks, Jews, or
African Americans (or women, the elderly, or liberals and conservatives) as if
we know all of them and they are all the same (Figure 13.4). These stereotypes
are, on their own, worrisome enough, because they can lead us to make serious
errors of judgment about the different people we meet. Worse, stereotypes can
lead to deeper problems, including (at the extreme) wars, genocides, and
“ethnic cleansings.” These larger calamities, however, are not fueled by
stereotypes alone. They arise from prejudice,
which can be defined as a negative attitude toward another per-son based on his
group membership. Prejudice consists of three factors sometimes referred to as
the ABCs of prejudice: an affective
(emotional) component, which leads us to view the other group as “bad”; a behavioral component, which includes our
ten-dencies to discriminate against other groups; and a cognitive component (the stereo-type itself ). Prejudice can lead
to extreme cruelties and injustices, making the study of intergroup bias in
general, and stereotypes and prejudice in particular, a topic of some urgency.
Whether
stereotypes are negative or positive, they often have deep historical roots and
are transmitted to each new generation both explicitly (“Never trust a . . .”)
and implic-itly (via jokes, caricatures, portrayals in movies, and the like). Thus,
in Western cul-tures, we hear over and over about athletic blacks, academic
Asians, moody women, and lazy Latinos, and, like it or not, these associations
eventually sink in and are likely to affect our behavior—regardless of whether
we believe that the association has any factual basis.
Other
factors also foster the creation and maintenance of stereotypes. Consider, for
example, the fact that most of us have a lot of exposure to people in our own
group, and as a result, we have ample opportunity to see that the group is
diverse and made up of unique individuals. We generally have less exposure to
other groups, and so, with little opportunity for learning, we are likely to
perceive the group as merely a mass of more or less similar people. This so-called
out-group homogeneity effect is
reflected in such statements as “All Asians are alike” or “All Germans are
alike.” The first statement is almost invariably made by a non-Asian; the
second, by a non-German.
Just
a few decades ago, many people did not hesitate to make derogatory comments in
public settings about blacks, or women, or Jews. This made studying stereotypes
relatively straightforward, in that researchers could use explicit measures, which involve some form of self-report, to
assess negative stereotypes about groups of individuals.
The
times have changed, however, and stereotyping has become much less socially
acceptable in most quarters of life. As a result, people are now much less
likely to endorse explicitly racist or sexist statements than they were in the
recent past. Does this mean stereotypes no longer matter? Unfortunately not.
Stereotypes still influence people’s behavior, but the effects are quite
subtle, often happening automatically, outside our awareness (Bargh, Chen,
& Burrows, 1996; but see also Cesario, Plaks, & Higgins, 2006).
To
assess these less overt stereotypes, researchers have begun to use implicit measures. For example, some
researchers have measured brain responses to stereotype-relevant stimuli (Ito,
Willadsen-Jensen, & Correll, 2007) or response times to stereotype-related
questions (Fazio, 1995). These implicit measures allow us to detect biases that
people might prefer to keep hidden. They also allow us to observe biases that
people don’t even realize they have—biases that, in fact, conflict with the
person’s explicit (conscious) beliefs. Thus, people who explicitly hold
egalitarian views may still have assumptions about various outgroup members,
unconsciously believing, for example, that African Americans are more likely
than whites to be criminals, or that Hispanics are more hot-tempered than
non-Hispanics (Blair & Banaji, 1996; Chen & Bargh, 1997; Kawakami,
Dion, & Dovidio, 1998; Wittenbrink, Judd, & Park, 1997). In fact, this
conflict between conscious beliefs and unconscious assumptions and associations
seems to occur rela-tively often, thanks to the fact that our implicit and
explicit views are shaped by differ-ent influences, and so can sometimes
contradict each other (e.g., Nosek, 2007; Rydell, McConnell, Mackie, &
Strain, 2006).
Another
commonly used means of detecting implicit assumptions and associations is the
Implicit Association Test (IAT; Greenwald et al., 2002; Greenwald, McGhee,
& Schwartz, 1998; Grenwald, Nosek, & Banaji, 2003).* In the classic
version of the IAT, people are asked to make two types of judgments: whether a
face they see on the com-puter screen is that of a black person or a white
person, and whether a word they see on the screen is a “good” word (e.g., love, joy, honest, truth) or a “bad”
word (e.g., poison,agony, detest,
terrible). This seems simple enough, but the trick here is that the trials
withfaces (black or white) are intermixed with the trials with words (good or
bad), and par-ticipants must use the same two keys on the computer for both
judgments. Thus, in one condition, participants must use one key to indicate
“black” if a black face is shown and the same key to indicate “good” if a good
word is shown; a different key is used for “white” (for face trials) and “bad”
(for word trials). In another condition, things are arranged differently: Now
participants use one key to indicate “black” and “bad” (for faces and words,
respectively) and the other key to indicate “white” and “good.”
This
experiment assesses how easily the participants can manage each of these links.
Do they have an easier time putting “white” and “good” together (and so using
the same key to indicate both) than putting “white” and “bad” together? It
turns out that the first combination is easier for white research
participants—and for many African American participants as well (Figure 13.5).
This seems to suggest that the participants arrive at the experiment already
primed to associate each race with a certain evaluation
and
respond more slowly when the experiment requires them to break that association
(see, for example, W. A. Cunningham, Preacher, & Banaji, 2001; Greenwald et
al., 2002; Greenwald et al., 1998; B. K. Payne, 2001).
Whether
implicit or explicit, stereotypes have multiple effects. They influence what we
believe about other people and how we act toward them. Perhaps worst of all,
stereo-types influence how the targets of our stereotypes act—so that,
specifically, the stereotype leads members of the targeted group to behave in a
way that confirms the stereotype. In this way, stereotypes create self-fulfilling prophecies.
In
a classic demonstration, Rosenthal and Jacobson (1968) told a group of
elementary-school teachers that some of their students were “bloomers” and
could be expected to show substantial increases in IQ in the year ahead.
Although these “bloomers” were ran-domly chosen, they in fact showed
substantial increases in their test scores over the course of the next year, an
effect apparently produced by the teachers’ expectations. Several fac-tors
probably contributed to this effect, including the greater warmth and
encouragement the teachers offered, the individualized feedback they provided,
and the increased number of opportunities they gave the children they expected
would do well (M. J. Harris & Rosenthal, 1985; for a cautionary note about
the reliability of these effects, see Jussim & Harber, 2005; R. Rosenthal,
1991, 2002; H. H. Spitz, 1999).
How
does this finding apply to stereotypes? Here, too, it turns out that people are
heavily influenced by others’ expectations—even when we are considering
expectations for a group, rather than
expectations for an individual. As we saw, this influence is plainly visible in
the way that stereotype threatinfluences performance on tests
designed to measure intellectual ability. In one study, for example,
experimenters gave intelligence tests to African American students. Before
taking the test, half of the students were led to think briefly about their
race; the other half were not. The result showed—remarkably—that the students
who thought about their race did less well on the test (Steele, 1998). In other
studies, women have been given math tests, and just before the test, half have
been led to think briefly about their gender. This reminder leads the women to
do more poorly on the test (Shih, Pittinsky, & Ambady, 1999).
What’s
going on in these studies? These reminders about group membership lead the test
takers to think about the stereotypes for their group—that African Americans
are unintelligent, or that women can’t do math—and this makes the test takers
anx-ious. They know that others expect them to do poorly, and they know that if
they do not perform well, they will just confirm the stereotype. These fears
distract the test takers, consume cognitive resources, and undermine their
performance—so that the stereo-type ends up confirming itself (Johns, Inzlicht,
& Schmader, 2008). In addition, the test takers may fear that the
stereotype is to some extent correct, and this fear may lead the test takers to
lower their expectations and not try as hard. This, too, would under-mine
performance, which in turn would confirm the ugly stereotype.
Related Topics
Privacy Policy, Terms and Conditions, DMCA Policy and Compliant
Copyright © 2018-2023 BrainKart.com; All Rights Reserved. Developed by Therithal info, Chennai.