Attribution
People
everywhere spend a lot of time and energy observing the people around them and
asking , “ Why did she (or he) do that?” Social psychologists call the process
of answering this question causal
attribution, and the study of how people form attri-butions is one of
social psychology ’s central concerns (see, for example, F. Heider, 1958; E. E.
Jones, & Nisbett, 1972; H. H. Kelley, 1967; H. H. Kelley & Michela,
1980). As we saw, thinking about the world often requires us to go beyond the
information we are actually given. We interpret the visual images on our
retinas, for example, by using top-down knowledge to supplement what we see. We
draw inferences from the observations we make, to reach broader conclusions
about the world. As we will see, similar intellectual activity is essential in
the social domain.
People
make attributions in roughly the same way that scientists track down the causes
of physical events (H. H. Kelley, 1967). For a scientist, an effect (such as an
increase in gas pressure) is attributed to a particular condition (such as a
rise in temperature) if the effect occurs when the condition is present but
does not occur when the condition is absent. In other words, the scientist
needs to know whether the cause and the effect covary. According to social
psychologist Harold Kelley, when people try to explain the behavior of others,
they use a similar covariation principle.
This
means that, to answer the question “Why did Mary smile at me?” we have to
consider when Mary smiles. Does she smile consistently whenever you walk into
the room? Does she refrain from smiling when others arrive? If the answer to
both of these questions is yes, then her smile does covary with your arrival,
and so is probably best understood as a result of her feelings about you. If it
turns out, though, that Mary smiles just as broadly when greeting others, then
we have to come up with a different explanation (F. Heider, 1958; H. H. Kelley,
1967).
Causal
attributions can be divided into two broad types—those that focus on factors
external to the person (e.g., Mary smiled because the situation demanded that
she be polite) and those that focus on the person herself (e.g., Mary smiled
because she is friendly). Explanations of the first type are called situational attributions and involve
factors such as other people’s expectations, the presence of rewards or
punishments, or even the weather. Explanations of the second type, dispositional attributions, focus on
factors that are internal to the person, such as traits, preferences, and other
personal qualities (Figure 13.1).
How
do people choose attributions for the behaviors they observe? Kelley ’s
proposal was that people are sensitive to the evidence they encounter, just as
a scientist would be, and draw their conclusions according to this evidence. It
turns out, how-ever, that this is not quite right, because people have strong
biases in the way they interpret the behavior of others, biases that can
sometimes lead them to overrule the evidence. These biases come from many
sources, including the culture in which someone lives.
Every
person is a part of many cultures—those defined by race, nationality, and
ethnicity, and also those defined by gender, socioeconomic status, sexual
preference, urbanicity (e.g., city dwelling or rural dwelling), economy (e.g.,
agricultural or industrial), and historical cohort (e.g., baby boomer or gen
Xer). This diversity means that cultures differ on many dimensions, but there
is reason to believe that one dimension is especially important—whether a
culture is more individualistic or more collectivistic (Triandis, 1989, 1994).
As
the name suggests, individualistic
cultures cater to the rights, needs, and preferences of the individual. The
majority cultures (e.g., middle-class, of European her-itage) of the United
States, western Europe, Canada, and Australia are individualistic (Figure
13.2). In these cultures, people tend to view themselves and others as
independent entities—that is, as fundamentally separate from others and their
environ-ment. They also generally think that people behave according to their
internal thoughts, feelings, needs, and preferences (A. P. Fiske, Kitayama,
Markus, & Nisbett, 1998; Markus & Kitayama, 1991), and not according to
outside influences, such as other people’s expec-tations or the demands of a
situation. To emphasize their independence and distinctive-ness, people in
individualistic cultures often strive to stand out by achieving personal goals.
They still feel obligated to their families and communities, but regularly
override these social obligations in order to pursue their own paths.
Collectivistic cultures, on the other hand, stress the importance of maintainingthe norms, standards, and traditions of families and other social groups. Most of the world’s cultures are collectivistic, including many of those of Latin America, Asia, and Africa. In collectivistic cultures, people tend to view themselves and others as interdependent—that is, as fundamentally connected to the people in their immedi-ate community and to their environment. They usually think that people behave according to the demands of a situation or the expectations of others, and not according to their personal preferences or proclivities. People still have their own dreams, desires, and life plans, of course, but they are more likely to create those plans according to the wishes and expectations of others, and to change them when the situation demands.
It
bears emphasizing that not everyone in collectivistic cultures has an
interdependent notion of the person, just as not everyone in individualistic
cultures has an independent notion. Instead, these terms describe what is
typical, as well as what each culture’s traditions, laws, religions, schools,
and media encourage.
These
differences among cultures influence us in many ways—including the ways we
think about other people’s behavior. In particular, people from
indi-vidualistic cultures routinely ascribe others’ behavior to dispositions
and not to situations—even when there is ample reason to believe that
situations are playing a crucial role (Figure 13.3). Thus North Americans of
European her-itage tend to see people on public assistance as lazy (a
dispositional attri-bution), for example, rather than struggling in an economy
with high unemployment and few entry-level positions (a situational
attribu-tion). Likewise, members of these cultures tend to view poor performance
on a test as a sign of low
intelligence(disposition) rather than as a result of an overly difficult exam
(situation). Thisbias is so pervasive that it is called the fundamental attribution error(D.T. Gilbert & Malone, 1995; L . Ross, 1977;
Sabini,Siepmann, & Stein, 2001).
To
dramatize this error, one early study had American college students
partici-pate in a simulated TV quiz show. Students were run in pairs and drew
cards to decide who would be the “quizmaster” and who the “contestant.” The
quizmaster had to make up questions, drawn from any area in which she had some
expertise; the contestant had to try to answer these questions. The game then
proceeded, and, inevitably, some of the quizmasters’ questions were extremely
difficult (e.g., “What do the initials W.
H. stand for in the poet W. H. Auden’s name?”).* A student audience watched
and subsequently rated how knowledgeable the two participants were.
The
situation plainly favored the quizmasters, who could choose any question or
topic they wished. Hence, if a quizmaster had knowledge of just one obscure
topic, he could focus all his questions on that topic, without revealing that
he had little knowledge in other domains. The contestants, on the other hand,
were at the mercy of whatever questions their quizmaster posed. Any
interpretation of the quizmasters’ “superiority” should take this obvious
situational advantage into account. But theobservers consistently failed to do
this. They knew that the roles in the setting—who was quizmaster, who was
contestant—had been determined by chance, for they had witnessed the entire
procedure. Even so, they could not help regarding the quizmas-ters as more
knowledgeable than the contestants—a tribute to the power of the fun-damental
attribution error (L .Ross, Amabile, & Steinmetz, 1977).
The
pattern of attributions is quite different, though, in collectivistic cultures.
In one study, Hindu Indians and European Americans were asked to discuss
vignettes about other people’s actions. Consistent with other research, the
European Americans’ comments included twice as many dispositional explanations
as situational explana-tions. The Hindu Indians showed the opposite pattern.
They gave twice as many situa-tional explanations as dispositional
explanations. As an illustration, one of the vignettes used in the study
described an accident in which the back wheel of a motor-cycle burst, throwing
the passenger off the motorcycle, and the driver had done little to help the
hurt passenger. Overall, the Americans typically described the driver as
“obvi-ously irresponsible” or “in a state of shock,” whereas the Indians
typically explained that it was the driver’s duty to be at work or that the
other person’s injury must not have looked serious (J. G. Miller, 1984; see
also A. P. Fiske et al., 1998; Maass, Karasawa, Politi, & Suga, 2006; P. B.
Smith & Bond, 1993).
Related Topics
Privacy Policy, Terms and Conditions, DMCA Policy and Compliant
Copyright © 2018-2023 BrainKart.com; All Rights Reserved. Developed by Therithal info, Chennai.