Home | | Psychology | Perception: Other Modalities

Chapter: Psychology: Perception

Perception: Other Modalities

Perception: Other Modalities
We’ve almost finished our discussion of perception, but before we bring the discussion to a close, there’s just one more topic to explore. We’ve exam-ined visual perception and how we recognize the things we see.

OTHER MODALITIES

We’ve almost finished our discussion of perception, but before we bring the discussion to a close, there’s just one more topic to explore. We’ve exam-ined visual perception and how we recognize the things we see. What about the other modalities? How do we hear—and understand—the speech that reaches our ears or the music we listen to? How do we perceive smells and tastes? How do we experience the warmth of the sun or the cold of an icy wind?

Of course, the various senses differ in many ways. Still, let’s not lose track of how much the different sense modalities have in common. For example, as we noted, all of the modalities are powerfully influenced by contrast effects, so sugar tastes sweeter after a sip of lemon juice, just as red seems more intense after looking at green. Likewise, each sense is influenced by the others—and so what we taste is influenced by the smells that are present, how a voice sounds depends on whether it’s coming from a face that looks friendly, and so on.

Many of the phenomena discussed also have direct parallels in other senses. For example, we’ve examined the perceiver’s role in organizing the visual input, and similar points can be made for hearing: Just as vision must parse the input, decid-ing where one object ends and the next begins, our sense of hearing must parse the stream of sounds, deciding where one note ends and the next begins or where one word ends and the next begins. And, just as in vision, the input for hearing is sometimes ambiguous—fully compatible with more than one parsing. This was evident, for exam-ple, in the mondegreens we discussed earily—including the celebrated misperception of Jimi Hendrix’s song “Purple Haze.” The song contained the lyric, “’Scuse me while I kiss the sky,” but many people mis-heard the lyric as “. . . while I kiss this guy.” The two key phrases (“the sky” and “this guy”) are virtually identical acoustically, and the difference between them is therefore “in the ear of the beholder”; no wonder, then, that the phrase was often misperceived.

Hearing, like vision, also requires us to group together separate elements in order to hear coherent sequences. This requirement is crucial, for example, for the perception of music. We must group together the notes in the melody line, perceiving them separately from the notes in the harmony, in order to detect the relationships from one note to the next within the melody; otherwise, the melody would be lost to us.

Likewise, our recognition of sounds seems to begin with the input’s acoustic features, just as the recognition of objects begins with the input’s visual features. Our analysis of sound features is supplemented by top-down (knowledge-driven) processing, just as it is for vision, and the relevant knowledge for hearing includes the hearer’s knowledge about the context as well as her broader knowledge about the world.

 

And hearing, like vision, is powerfully influenced by attention (Figure 5.39). Indeed, some of the classic work on attention is done with auditory inputs and yields data con-sistent with the findings we reviewed for vision: When our attention is focused on one auditory input, we hear remarkably little from other inputs. Thus, if we’re listening to one conversation at a cocktail party, we’ll be able to hear that the unattended voice behind us is loud rather than soft, a male’s rather than a female’s; but we’ll have no idea what the unattended voice is actually saying or even whether the unattended speech


makes any sense at all. This point has been documented by studies that require partic-ipants to pay attention to a voice arriving (through headphones) in one ear, while another (unattended) voice is presented to the other ear. In this setup, participants grasp virtually none of the content of the unattended message. In one classic study, the participants couldn’t even tell whether the speaker on the unattended ear was reading the passage backward or forward (Cherry, 1953; Reisberg, 2010).

But let’s be clear that participants aren’t totally deaf to an unattended message. If, for example, this message contains the participants’ own names, a substantial number of par-ticipants will detect this significant stimulus (Moray, 1959). Participants may also detect other personally relevant stimuli, such as the name of a favorite restaurant or the title of a book they’ve just read (Conway, Cowan, & Bunting, 2001; Wood & Cowan, 1995). The reason for this is priming. The participant has presumably encountered these significant stimuli recently, so the relevant detectors are probably still “warmed up” and will respond even to a weak input. Just as in vision, therefore, the perception of a stimulus can be pro-moted either by expectations or by recent exposure; and either form of priming can offset the significant disadvantages associated with the absence of attention.

In short, then, many of the lessons we’ve learned about vision can be applied directly to the sense of hearing. This is true for many of our specific claims about vision; it’s also true for our methodological claims—particularly the need for multiple levels of descrip-tion. In hearing no less than in vision, it’s immensely useful to approach the problems of perception in more than one way, with a close examination of function (what the processes achieve) and the biology (how the processes are implemented in the nervous system) if we are to understand how people come to know the world around them.

Study Material, Lecturing Notes, Assignment, Reference, Wiki description explanation, brief detail
Psychology: Perception : Perception: Other Modalities |


Privacy Policy, Terms and Conditions, DMCA Policy and Compliant

Copyright © 2018-2024 BrainKart.com; All Rights Reserved. Developed by Therithal info, Chennai.