The Logic of
Psychometrics
We have before us two hypotheses
concerned with the nature of intelligence, and the way we’ve described the
hypotheses points toward the means of deciding which hypothesis is correct.
What we need to do is take a closer look at the IQ tests them-selves and try to
find patterns within the test scores. This kind of scrutiny reflects the psychometric approach to intelligence—an
approach that, at the start, deliberatelyholds theory and definitions to the
side. Instead, it begins with the actual test results and proceeds on the
belief that patterns within these results may be our best guide in deciding
what the tests measure—and therefore what intelligence is. To see how this
works, let’s begin with a hypothetical example.
Imagine that we give a group of
individuals three tests that seem at least initially dif-ferent from each
other; let’s call the tests X, Y, and Z. One hypothesis is that all three tests
measure the same underlying ability—and so, if a person has a lot of this
ability, she has what she needs for all three tests and will do well on all of
them. (This is, of course, the idea that there’s a general ability used for many different tasks.) Based on this
hypothesis, a person’s score on one of these tests should be similar to her
score on the other tests because, whatever the level of ability happens to be,
it’s the same ability that matters
for all three tests. This hypothesis therefore leads to a prediction that there
will be a strong correlation between each person’s score on test X and his or
her score on test Y, and the same goes for X and Z or for Y and Z.
A different hypothesis is that
each of tests X, Y, and Z measures a different abil-ity. This is, of course,
the idea that there’s no such thing as ability in general; instead, each person
has their own collection of more specialized capacities. Based on this view,
it’s possible for a person to have the ability needed for X but not the
abilities needed for Y or Z. It’s also possible for a person to have the
abilities needed for both Y and Z but not those needed for X . It’s possible
for a person to have all of these abilities or none of them. In short, all combinations are possible because
we’re talking about three separate abilities; thus, there is no reason to
expect a cor-relation between someone’s X score and his Y score, or between his
Y score and his Z score, just as there’s no reason to expect a correlation
between, say, someone’s ability to knit well and the size of his vocabulary.
Knitting and knowing a lot of words are simply independent capacities—so you
can be good at one, or the other, or both, or neither.
We could also imagine
intermediary hypotheses: Perhaps X and Y tap into the same, somewhat general
ability, but Z taps into some other, more specialized ability. Perhaps Y and Z
overlap in the abilities relevant for each, but the overlap is only partial.
Throughout, however, the logic is the same: If two tests overlap in the
abilities they
require, then they are to some
extent measuring the same thing; so there should be a cor-relation between the
scores on the two tests. If the overlap is slight, the correlation will be
weak; if the overlap is greater, the correlation will be correspondingly
stronger. (For a slightly different—and somewhat whimsical—view of these
points, see Figure 11.5.)
Related Topics
Privacy Policy, Terms and Conditions, DMCA Policy and Compliant
Copyright © 2018-2023 BrainKart.com; All Rights Reserved. Developed by Therithal info, Chennai.