Brain Organization and Language
Development
It is thought that if language is indeed an innate
capacity, there must be a neural basis for this capacity that is present from
birth. In this regard, there has been some debate about the concepts of
equipotentiality and differential lateralization at birth.
Equipotentiality of the brain for language means
that both sides are capable of performing linguistic functions.
Equipotentiality also suggests plasticity of the brain and a ca-pacity for
reorganization even after injury so that language functions can be transferred
from one hemisphere to the other. Indeed, lesion data for childhood aphasia and
earlier reports of hemispherectomy studies have suggested functional recovery
with respect to language until puberty, as evidenced by ipsilat-eral and
contralateral transfer of linguistic functions. Thus, it has been reported that
if there is damage to the left side in infancy but the right side remains
intact, the child can still develop normal language (Curtiss, 1989; Dennis and
Whitaker, 1976).
In early childhood, after the onset of language but
before age 4 years, damage to the language areas results in transient aphasia.
After puberty, by about age 14 years, the prognosis begins to worsen, and
similar lesions in adulthood can cause irreversible deficits (Lenneberg, 1967,
1969). Some authors, such as Lenneberg (1967), have taken these findings about
the capacity for transfer of function in young children to mean that the brain
is symmetrically organized to begin with and only gradually becomes
asymmetri-cally specialized, resulting in a diminished capacity for recovery
with age. However, there is now increasing evidence that the brain is
asymmetrical with respect to linguistic ability from birth.
If the brain is lateralized for linguistic
functions from birth, a question arises about the evidence for greater
recoverability of language when damage occurs early enough in life. One
interpre-tation of this recoverability is that there is greater neuroplasticity
in the young brain, as evident from the capacity of surviving neu-rons to make
new synaptic connections even after injury.
Lenneberg’s view of the critical period for
language devel-opment (from birth to the early teens) was that both hemispheres
are involved in language functions at first, but by puberty the left becomes
more specialized. A later view relates the concept of critical period to the
notion of neural plasticity. In any case, it is thought that the critical
period is correlated with innate mecha-nisms, and that language development is
most susceptible to the limiting effects of both biological and environmental
factors if these extend beyond this critical period.
Related Topics
Privacy Policy, Terms and Conditions, DMCA Policy and Compliant
Copyright © 2018-2023 BrainKart.com; All Rights Reserved. Developed by Therithal info, Chennai.