Home | | Psychiatry | Major Subdivisions of Memory Systems

Chapter: Essentials of Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuropsychology

Major Subdivisions of Memory Systems

Major Subdivisions of Memory Systems
Essentials of Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuropsychology

Major Subdivisions of Memory Systems

 

Current research on the cognitive neuroscience of memory has considered memory from the perspective of multiple systems rather than a unitary system. Figure 15.4 illustrates the subdi-visions between memory systems. A number of systems have been proposed, and the distinction between these systems is not impermeable. The most basic distinction between memory sys-tems is that of explicit versus implicit memory. Explicit memory is also referred to as declarative memory and implicit memory is also known as nondeclarative memory. What is considered explicit about this type of memory is that it requires conscious awareness of past experience (Cohen and Squire, 1980). There are two major components of explicit memory: episodic memory and semantic memory. Episodic or autobiographical memory is the

 

 

ability to remember personal events over time. This refers to the individual’s ability to remember not only that something occurred but also the context in which it occurred. Semantic memory, in contrast, refers to knowledge without context. Another type of explicit memory system that involves the short-term registering of information is termed working memory. Memory systems research in the past several years has also focused on implicit memory. Implicit memory does not involve conscious awareness. It refers to the effects of previous experience on cur-rent behavior without conscious recollection.

 

On the basis of cases of medial temporal lobe damage in humans, it has been concluded that the medial temporal lobe, including the hippocampus, and adjacent anatomical structures enable the formation of explicit memories. In humans, it has been shown that a lesion confined solely to the hippocampus (field CA1 of the hippocampus) can produce a mild amnestic syndrome (Zola-Morgan et al., 1986). It would then appear that the para-hippocampal and perirhinal cortical regions are also necessar-ily involved in explicit memory. According to Zola-Morgan and Squire (1993), the medial temporal lobe system essentially coor-dinates the organization of information that originated in other brain regions. The medial temporal lobe system may thereby act as a temporary site to store information that is cortically distrib-uted until the information is permanently coded.

 

Diencephalon and Explicit Memory

 

Neuropsychological studies of patients with Wernicke–Korsakoff disease have yielded insight into the contribution of the dien-cephalon (dorsal and anterior thalamic nuclei and the mamillary bodies of the hypothalamus) to memory. Wernicke–Korsakoff disease is usually associated with a thiamine deficiency and is typically seen in individuals with alcoholism, nutritional defi-ciencies, infections and brain tumors (Markowitsch and Pritzel, 1985). The memory deficit is characterized by impairment of ex-plicit memory as seen by dense anterograde amnesia and variable retrograde amnesia. These patients are also frequently apathetic and indifferent and have diminished initiative.

The effects of damage caused by alcoholic and nonalco-holic lesions to the anterior and medial thalamic nuclei as well as to the mamillary bodies are diverse. The diencephalic region is connected with the hippocampal region; the mamillary bodies are connected to the hippocampus via the fornix and to the an-terior thalamus via the mamillothalamic tract. The mediodorsal thalamic component of the system is also interconnected with the frontal lobes. Patients with Korsakoff’s disease typically mani-fest frontal lobe damage, which is seen in the phenomenon of confabulation. Aside from this frontal component, which is often seen in alcoholism and may represent an independent lesion, the diencephalic amnesia is largely similar to the kinds of memory deficits observed with damage to the medial temporal lobe sys-tem. This suggests that, at least at a functional level, anatomical damage in this subcortical system produces an impairment in the consolidation of information.

 

Basal Forebrain and Memory

 

Another neuroanatomical region that plays a direct role in ex-plicit memory is the basal forebrain. The basal forebrain region is located where the diencephalon meets the cerebral hemispheres and includes a number of brain structures such as the septal area, diagonal band of Broca, nucleus accumbens septi, olfactory tu-bercle, and substantia innominata. Figure 15.5 illustrates the ros-tral cholinergic projections in the rat brain. The contribution of this brain region to memory has been a relatively recent discov-ery. The cholinergic system plays a role in memory (although its full role is not clear at this time), and is implicated in normal and pathological aging, but is only one locus in the network of brain regions involved in memory.

 

Affective Valence and Neuromodulatory Systems

 

Memory functioning requires a system for establishing va-lence between memorable events. That is, some events are more memorable than others. Affect and its associated chemical neuromodulators probably serve a valence capacity by facilitating the storage of emotionally charged experiences. The underlying assumption here is that emotionally tinged experiences activate neurobiological pathways that facilitate their storage. In evolu-tionary terms, it is highly adaptive to remember experiences that are learned under arousing conditions. Neuroimaging studies have provided evidence for the role of the amygdala in forming declarative memories. A number of neuromodulators play a role in affective responding and also influence memory storage. Chief among them is the noradrenergic system. The central noradrener-gic system involves the locus coeruleus in the midbrain reticular formation, the amygdala and the stria terminalis (a major afferent and efferent pathway to the amygdala). A number of findings have converged to suggest that central norepinephrine (NE) receptors in the amygdala are involved in the postlearning consolidation of information (Liang et al., 1986).

 

Overall, the role of the noradrenergic and other systems, such as GABA and opioid peptides, in memory may lie in the re-lease of adrenal epinephrine after stressful or emotional stimuli. Activation of NE in the locus coeruleus–amygdala system, which ultimately has extensive connectivity with a number of brain re-gions, then serves to consolidate the storage of these memories. On a cellular level this may occur by the production of LTP, be-cause noradrenergic compounds have been found to enhance LTP (Gold et al., 1984).

Study Material, Lecturing Notes, Assignment, Reference, Wiki description explanation, brief detail
Essentials of Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuropsychology : Major Subdivisions of Memory Systems |


Privacy Policy, Terms and Conditions, DMCA Policy and Compliant

Copyright © 2018-2023 BrainKart.com; All Rights Reserved. Developed by Therithal info, Chennai.