Neurodevelopmental Aspects of
Language
Any description of the relationship between the
brain and language would be incomplete without considering some
neurodevelopmental aspects of language. As a comprehensive dis-cussion of this
subject is beyond the scope of this section, the fo-cus here is on presenting
central theoretical issues. Some of these issues help shed light on the ways in
which the brain is organized for language in children. For discussions of
language disorders in children, the reader is referred to Yule and Rutter
(1987).
One central issue addresses the notion of the
innateness of lan-guage. Interest in this concept was stimulated by the
pioneeringwork of Chomsky (1988), who espoused the view of an in-nate human
capacity to know the universal rules of grammar. Chomsky drew attention to the
fact that despite differences in the languages of the world, all human
languages have the same universal features. Consequently, learning a language
is some-thing that “happens” to a child (Goldberg, 1989).
The fact that there are universal regularities in
the acquisi-tion of language supports the notion of an innate capacity. Thus,
according to Lenneberg (1967, 1969), an infant’s language capa-bilities are
linked with physical maturation and there is therefore a correlation between
language development and motor develop-ment. Thus, by about 15 months of age,
when the motor milestone of self-propulsive gait is attained, an infant has a
vocabulary of three to 50 words; by 18 to 24 months, when a child begins to run
(with falls), many two-word utterances are observed (Lenneberg, 1969; Brown,
1973). By about 3 to 4 years of age, a child ac-quires the capability for many
fully grammatical utterances (Stromswold, 1995).
In the literature dealing with the issue of
language acquisi-tion, much consideration has been given to the relative
influences of genetic and environmental factors. Individual differences in performance
raise questions about the relative contributions of these factors in language
acquisition. On the one hand, it has been thought that if language is indeed an
innate capacity with associated neural mechanisms, then language functions and
mal-functions must also have a genetic basis.
Evidence for a genetic basis has come from many
sources, especially from research in the area of developmental language
disorders. Based on a review of relevant studies, Stromswold (1995) reported
the finding of a higher incidence of language impairment in families of
children with developmental disorders than in families of children without such
impairment.
With respect to the view of language as an innate
capacity, it is generally believed that language can be acquired without
ex-plicit instruction. For example, Stromswold (1995) reported that even
children who are unable to speak and therefore cannot be corrected are capable
of acquiring normal receptive language. In addition to the evidence that
supports genetic contributions to language functions and dysfunctions, there is
indirect support for the role of environmental exposure from studies of
individu-als raised in severely deprived environments. Studies of “wild”
children raised in conditions of extreme linguistic, social and emotional
deprivation essentially suggested an innate hypothesis for language acquisition
but also suggested that the environment can have some modifying influences.
Related Topics
Privacy Policy, Terms and Conditions, DMCA Policy and Compliant
Copyright © 2018-2023 BrainKart.com; All Rights Reserved. Developed by Therithal info, Chennai.