Theories and Models of Development
Theories are important to the study of development
for a number of reasons. They organize and prioritize large amounts of data
regarding infant development, indicating which are the most sa-lient and why.
Often, they also explain the importance of the early years for subsequent
development, indicating how developmen-tal issues are related to broader issues
of the lifespan. Generally, they move beyond mere descriptions of behavior and
attempt to explain why individuals are motivated to behave in certain ways at
certain times. Finally, they may generate meaningful and test-able hypotheses
for empirical research.
On the other hand, theories have inherent
liabilities. A selective focus on one theory may obscure others of equal or
greater value. Theories also inevitably lead to oversimplification of complex
processes and events. They may create biases that af-fect how we interpret
observations and how we make inferences from these observations. The history of
psychology is filled with examples of adherence to a particular point of view,
making it impossible to see disconfirming information. All of these factors
warrant caution about the uncritical use of theories to understand development.
A useful theory is one which is developmental, in-tegrative, contextualist,
constructionist and perspectivist in na-ture, discounting its own absolute
claim to truth and integrating as many relevant approaches as possible. An
integrative devel-opmental theory accounts for the dynamic interactions of
biol-ogy (including neuroanatomy, genetics, neurotransmitters, etc.),
relationships (including parental, sibling, peer and wider social
groups), culture (including cultural norms for
individual and col-lective development), and technology (including medication,
in-formatics, etc.) (Wilber, 2000).
Table 7.2 presents a brief summary of some of the
major theories of development as they pertain to the first 3 years of life.
Although others could have been selected, those presented have been most
influential with regard to clinical practice and research on early development.
As noted in Table 7.2, the theories vary with regard to their particular focus
of development, although most use stages to describe periods of discontinuity.
There has been a scientific preoccupation with
defining the relative contributions of genetic endowment and environmental
experience on the course of human development. While early be-havioralists took
the extreme view that children can be shaped al-most exclusively by their
environments, today evidence supports the view that genes and experience
interact continuously overtime in a transactional manner that leads to the
unique develop-ment of an individual. The study of environmental contributions
has steadily improved through the application of more careful methods of
assessment and an appreciation of the value of exam-ining the many components
of the early experience of children.
However, the most explosive advances in the
understand-ing of human development have been made as specific gene sequences
have been identified and linked to physical and be-havioral outcomes. In the
past decade, the pace of new gene discovery has increased exponentially as a
consequence of the success of the Human Genome Project. It is now estimated
that human beings have approximately 38 000 to 40 000 genes. Deter-mining the
precise number has been elusive as it has been neces-sary gradually to
understand that human genes are more complex than the genes of simpler
organisms. Specifically, many human genes produce multiple proteins. We are now
learning about the degrees of genomic variability that exist between
individuals, as well as beginning to understand how genes interact with each
other and how they are regulated by the environment. A key fo-cus of new
research is the discovery of how the passage of time and the gradual maturation
of the individual affect the expression of genes that have remained silent but
potentially ominous from the beginning of fetal development. Future studies of
cohorts of infants who are at known genetic risk for a trait or illness maywell
identify environmental factors associated with both the ex-pression and the
suppression of gene expression.
The concept of studying development longitudinally
has its origin in the studies of lives and was well established by Plu-tarch
and popularized by Shakespeare. In many ways, biogra-phers strive to examine
the origins of adult traits through consid-eration of the early experiences of
their particular subject. This tradition was adopted by psychoanalysts who
searched for the origins of psychopathology through the exposition of a
“genetic formulation’’. The choice of the word “genetic’’ to modify a
con-ceptual formulation based on the experience of the individual is somewhat
ironic. The term has largely been abandoned, as these formulations had little
to do with the function of individual genes. Nonetheless, this focus on the
influence of early experi-ence on development may well have been a
foreshadowing of the importance of intense early experience on gene expression.
In all likelihood, the genetic formulations of the future will focus on how
experience regulates gene expression at the molecular level.
The concept of parallel yet interacting lines of
develop-ment was popularized by Anna Freud (1946) who created a classical
monograph that articulated nine lines of development that were well described
through adolescence. Although some of these conceptual lines have been
abandoned, the overarching principle of a line of development has proven to
have heuristic value. Table 7.3 lists four relevant developmental lines.
Other psychoanalysts have built on her model to
create parallel lines extending into adulthood. Erikson (1963) further
elaborated the evolution of domains of function in the creation of his
epigenetic stage model, as presented in Figure 7.1. His para-digm continues to
have a strong influence on psychiatric theory.
as is wellillustrated by Vaillant’s work who has
extended the Eriksonian model by proposing two additional phases of the adult
life-cycle, career-consolidation vs. self-absorption and keeper of the meaning
vs. rigidity. The former acknowledges the importance of achieving a stable
career identity in addition to the achievement of identity within one’s family
of origin. The latter describes fur-ther Erikson’s concept of generativity
beyond assuming sustained responsibility for building the community and for the
growth, well-being and leadership of others. “Keeper of the meaning’’ and its
virtue, wisdom, involve a nonpartisan and less personal approach to others and
is to be distinguished from the tasks of a generative coach, partisan parent,
or mentor from the tasks of a Supreme Court judge or chair of a historical
society.
Although lines of development are attractive
conceptually, they are a deceptively simplistic representation of the complex
evolution of personality. The concept of “decalage’’ was put for-ward by Piaget
(1952) to refer to a disengagement in the normal evolution of the parallel
development of specific cognitive abili-ties. However, decalage is equally
salient in the conceptualiza-tion of major distortions in emotional or social
development.
Related Topics
Privacy Policy, Terms and Conditions, DMCA Policy and Compliant
Copyright © 2018-2023 BrainKart.com; All Rights Reserved. Developed by Therithal info, Chennai.