Cognitive Developmen
The study of cognitive development provides a
perspective on the evolution of the capacity to think. Increased cognitive
abili-ties are an integral component required for the onset of language.
Changes in thinking ultimately shape the course of emotional, social and moral
development. Recent investigations of the re-lation of brain development to
cognitive development have at-tempted to attribute specific developmental
changes in central nervous system function with the achievement of new
cognitive abilities (Casey et al.,
2000; Anderson, 2001).
However, the acquisition of mental abilities has
been charted as an independent sequence of mental accomplishments. Piaget
es-tablished the field of cognitive development, and his stage theory of the
evolution of cognitive processes has dominated this field (Piaget and Inhelder,
1969). Although specific aspects of his four primary stages have been modified
by subsequent empirical experi-ments as well as by the development of a greater
appreciation of the role of emotions and context in the utilization of
cognitive abilities, his careful observations and brilliant deductions have
provided the framework on which much of our knowledge of cognitive develop-ment
has been built. In contrast, Vygotsky (1978) provided a model of early
cognitive development that placed greater importance on the influence of
culture and language-mediated guidance by adults
Piaget introduced the concept of “schemas’’ which
he defi ned as units of cognition. He further described processes that result
in schema modifi cation, which begin in infancy as a child assimilates new
information and accommodates to novel stimuli. A particularly important
Piagetian concept has been that of a decalage within cognitive development,
which refers to an unevenness in cognitive development. For example, a child
may demonstrate cognitive abilities at the concrete operational stage of development with regard to conservation of
volume and at the same time retain preoperational forms of thinking such as
per-sistent egocentrism. Such an unevenness can also be seen across lines of
development.
Even newborns have the ability to learn through
making associations between different states or experiences. There is evidence
that cognitive “prewiring’’ exists, which allows for the perceptual capacities
of infants that are necessary to seek stimu-lation and interaction with adult
caregivers. A key capacity re-quired for these early cognitions is recognition
of the invariant features of perceptual stimuli coupled with the ability to
translate these invariant features across sensory modalities. Interestingly,
infants can differentiate the human voice from other sounds in-nately without
“learning’’ the complex characteristics of the structure and pitch of speech.
By 2 to 3 weeks of age, cross-modal fluency is
demon-strated by the ability of infants to imitate facial expression. This
requires the recognition of a visual schema of a facial expression to be linked
with a proprioceptive tactile schema of producing a facial expression. By 3
months of age, infants can be classically conditioned, and their interest in
stimuli led Piaget to suggest this was a period dominated by attempts to make
“interesting spectacles last’’ (Piaget and Inhelder, 1969).
By 6 months of age, associations between “means’’
and “ends’’ have been demonstrated. This is followed by object per-manence,
which evolves during the second half of the first year. During the second year,
infants can infer cause after observing an effect as well as anticipate effects
after producing a causal action. A corollary of this new ability is that they
are now able correctly to sequence past events.
By the third year of life, children enter the
preoperational stage. This form of thinking is more similar to adult cognition,
but it incorporates magical explanations and is marked by a tendency to focus
on one perceptual attribute at a time (centra-tion). Idiosyncratic cosmological
theories are common and are usually dominated by transductive reasoning, which
attributes causality based exclusively on temporal or spatial juxtaposition.
Throughout the preschool period, attention span and memory are limited while
pretend play and fanciful thinking are common. Animism is frequently used and
refers to endowing inanimate objects with the qualities of living things. Not
surprisingly, chil-dren having imaginary friends and talking pets characterize
this cognitive period. The preoperational stage is also the time dur-ing which
explosive language development occurs. This develop-ment appears to be made
possible by a genetically determined capacity for language. However, language
development is clearly enhanced by experiential support and parental
communication that is sensitive to the child’s ability to process new words and
grammatical structure.
By age 6 or 7 years, children begin to use
operational think-ing. The child who has attained concrete operational thought has
the ability to conserve both volume and quantity as well as being able to
appreciate the reversibility of events and ideas. A shift from an egocentric
perspective results in a new capacity to appre-ciate the perspective of others.
These new cognitive skills dem-onstrate an ability to engage in logical
dialogue and to develop an appreciation of more complex causal sequences. These
new cognitive abilities are clearly required for a child to benefit from the
grade school curriculum.
Adolescence results in the development of a new
processing capacity that involves the manipulation of ideas and
conceptsFurthermore, the informational fund of knowledge is dramatically
expanded and serves as a referent for verification of new data that are
assimilated. A final major transition is possible with the development of the
ability to reflect on cognition as a process. This is referred to as the
development of a metacognitive capacity. Achievement of this capacity allows
adolescents to understand and empathize with the divergent perspectives of
others to a greater degree than was previously possible. This capacity is
necessary for recursive thinking which involves an awareness that others can
think about the domain of the adolescent’s own thought. These cognitive skills represent
the transition into the final stage of cognitive ability, which is referred to
as the use of formal operations. One capacity characteristic of this stage is
the ability to understand complex combinatorial systems that require a well-developed
sense of reversibilities that include inversion, reciprocity and symmetry. New
levels of problem solving are achieved that include the ability to recognize a
core problem or core isomorph within a more complex new problem. The adult with
formal operational ability can recognize a previously successful solution and
use this knowledge to develop a parallel innovative solution to the complex
problem. It is important to appreciate that many adults remain at the stage of
concrete operations and never develop these more advanced capacities.
A timeline of cognitive development during the
course of the lifespan is presented in Figure 7.3.
Related Topics
Privacy Policy, Terms and Conditions, DMCA Policy and Compliant
Copyright © 2018-2023 BrainKart.com; All Rights Reserved. Developed by Therithal info, Chennai.