Home | | Psychology | The Evolutionary Basis for Emotional Remembering

Chapter: Psychology: Prologue: What Is Psychology?

The Evolutionary Basis for Emotional Remembering

The Evolutionary Basis for Emotional Remembering
It seems, then, that the structure of the brain provides one reason that people remember emotional events so well—our memory for these events is directly shaped by a specific interaction among the brain’s parts.

The Evolutionary Basis for Emotional Remembering

It seems, then, that the structure of the brain provides one reason that people remember emotional events so well—our memory for these events is directly shaped by a specific interaction among the brain’s parts. This aspect of the brain’s “wiring” is part of our biological heritage and is well established at birth—and so, apparently, it’s specified largely by the blueprint laid out in our genes. But where does this blue-print come from? The answer, in brief, is evolution: Our ancient ancestors varied somewhat in their biological traits; and, thanks to this variation, some of our ancestors were better suited to their environment than others. These better-suited ancestors were therefore more likely to survive than their less well-adapted neigh-bors, and so they were more likely to reproduce and so more likely to pass on their genetic material to the next generation. As this process then repeated itself, genera-tion after generation, the genetic pattern passed onward by these better-adapted ancestors became more and more common in our species. Consequently, modern humans are likely to have inherited this genetic pattern as well as the traits that it produces.

Why might evolution have shaped our memory for emotional events? One answer to this question starts with the sorts of emotional events our ancient ancestors were likely to experience. Encounters with dangerous predators would probably have filled our ancestors with fear, and they would surely have considered these encounters worth remembering so they could avoid facing the predator again. Likewise, the discovery of a site with especially plentiful berries might have been exciting—and this discovery, too, would be worth remembering, so that these ancient humans would be able to find the berries again. Examples like these remind us that it’s often useful to remember emo-tional events, because they typically involve experiences and information that truly matter for us. It seems plausible, therefore, that those of our ancestors who were espe-cially able to remember emotional events might have gained a survival advantage—a key element in evolution by natural selection.

In fact, evolution might have tuned our capacity to remember in fairly specific ways. To understand this point, consider a procedure known as fear conditioning: An organism is given a warning stimulus (a bright light, perhaps, or a tone) and then, a few seconds later, is presented with some noxious stimulus (e.g., the sight of a predator, or a painfully loud noise). With this setup, the organism quickly learns the significance of the warning stimulus, and becomes fearful the moment it begins. This is a good thing , because the signal—and the organism’s understanding of the signal—gives the animal time to prepare (or to flee) before the noxious stimulus arrives.

Not surprisingly, fear conditioning is slower if the organism has suffered damage to the amygdala—confirming the importance of this brain structure in supporting emo-tional memory. For present purposes, though, the crucial thing is that the speed of fear conditioning depends on the nature of the warning stimulus. A range of organisms, including humans, show faster learning if the warning stimulus is a picture of a snake than if it’s a picture of, say, a mushroom (Figure P.8). In other words, the pairing of “see picture of snake, then receive electric shock” produces more rapid learning than a pair-ing of “see a picture of a mushroom, then receive shock.” This pattern holds even if the


individual organism has never in its life seen a snake. It seems, then, that some organisms are prepared, in advance of any learning, to associate bad consequences with the sight of a snake. This response is almost surely a result of our evolutionary past, in which rapid learning about snakes mattered for survival.

Amazingly, this pattern of faster learning in response to snake pictures remains in place even if the snake picture, as the warning stimulus, is flashed so quickly on a com-puter screen that it’s virtually unrecognizable. This finding suggests that it’s not just learning that prepares us for these stimuli, but perception—which allows us to detect these (apparently threatening) creatures even from a split-second view.

Study Material, Lecturing Notes, Assignment, Reference, Wiki description explanation, brief detail
Psychology: Prologue: What Is Psychology? : The Evolutionary Basis for Emotional Remembering |


Privacy Policy, Terms and Conditions, DMCA Policy and Compliant

Copyright © 2018-2023 BrainKart.com; All Rights Reserved. Developed by Therithal info, Chennai.