A Developmental
Perspective on Emotional Memory
Clearly, then, if we are going to
understand how people remember emotional events, we need to consider many
factors and take several different perspectives. Some of these perspectives are
biological, including a microscopic view of specific changes taking place in
the brain when memories are formed and a macroscopic view emphasizing the
influence of evolution on brain function. A cognitive perspec-tive focuses on
which aspects of a situation people pay attention to and what kinds of knowledge
they merge with their memories of an experience. A social perspective leads us
to ask how and why people share their memories, and how such interac-tions
affect what is remembered. We also need to think about the ways that the
cul-tural context influences the sharing of memories and shapes how someone
reacts to (and recalls) the target event.
Yet another perspective is developmental: How do children learn to
recall emotional events? How do they learn the appropriate way, in their
culture, to report on events? How accurate or complete is their recollection?
These questions have significant scien-tific interest as well as considerable
practical importance. It’s increasingly common for children to provide evidence
for legal disputes, including divorce proceedings or trials involving
allegations of child abuse. In these settings, it is crucial to have some
guide-lines about how best to draw accurate information from children—and how
to evaluate the information children provide.
Fortunately for the courts,
children’s memories of events are often quite accurate and relatively complete.
But children are also highly suggestible—so, when asking them about past
events, it is crucial not to convey expectations that could influence their
answers. In many cases, children also do not understand which aspects of an
event are likely to be rel-evant to their audience, so they often need guidance
in choosing what to report.
One line of research, for
example, has examined how children remember events that are not just emotional,
but traumatic. Obviously, we cannot create these events in the laboratory—that
would be ethically repugnant. But outside of the lab, children some-times need
medical procedures that can be deeply upsetting or uncomfortable—includ-ing
procedures that involve medical professionals closely examining and
manipulating the young child’s genitals. How will they remember these
procedures later?
Evidence suggests that children
generally remember these medical procedures quite well. If questioned carefully
and neutrally, they can report many aspects of the event.
The one group of children least
likely to recall the event is, not surprisingly, those who are very young—for
example, children under three years old. These children probably lack the
intellectual and neural capacities to establish clear and explicit memories of
any sort; and so, inevitably, they cannot recall medical trauma.
However, these painful memories
are susceptible to the same influences as other memories. With time, even
traumatic memories fade and, as a result, are more and more open to errors. The
child may even recall elements of the event that never happened at all.
Likewise, if a child is questioned in a suggestive manner, her recollection can
be sub-stantially altered—aspects of the event may change, or new (fictional)
experiences might be introduced into the episode.
Related Topics
Privacy Policy, Terms and Conditions, DMCA Policy and Compliant
Copyright © 2018-2026 BrainKart.com; All Rights Reserved. Developed by Therithal info, Chennai.