Cognitive
Influences on Emotional Memory
It is clear, then, that our
understanding of emotional memory must include its biological basis—the neural
mechanisms that promote emotional memory, and the evolutionary heritage that
makes it easier for us to form some memories than oth-ers. But our theorizing
also needs to include the ways that people think
about the emotional events they experience: What do they pay attention to
during the experi-ence? How do they make sense of the emotional event, and how
does this interpre-tation shape their memory?
The role of attention is evident,
for example, in the fact that emotional memory tends to be uneven—some aspects
of the event are well remembered, other aspects are neglected. Thus, during a
robbery, witnesses might focus on the robbers themselves— what they did, what
they said (Figure P.9). As a result, the witnesses might remember these
“central” aspects of the event later on but might have little memory for other
aspects of the event—such as what the other witnesses were doing. In this
regard, memory is very different from, say, the sort of record created by a
videocamera. If the camera is turned on and functioning properly, it records
everything in front of the lens. Memory, in contrast, is selective; it records
the bits that someone was paying attention to, but nothing more. As a result,
memory—for emotional events and in general—is invariably incomplete. It can
sometimes omit information that is crucial for some purposes (e.g., information
the police might need when investigating the robbery).
Memory is also different from a
videorecord in another way. A video-camera is a passive device, simply
recording what is in front of the lens. In contrast, when you “record”
information into memory, you actively interpret the event, integrating
information gleaned from the event with other knowledge. Most of the time, this
is a good thing because it creates a rich and sophisticated memory trace that
preserves the event in combination with your impressions of the event as well
as links to other related episodes. However, this active interpretation of an
event also has a downside: People often lose track of the source of particular bits of information in memory. Specifically,
they lose track of which bits are drawn from the original episode they
experienced and which bits they supplied through their understanding of the
episode.
In one study, for example, people spent a few minutes in a profes-sor ’s office; then, immediately afterward, they were taken out of the office and asked to describe the room they had just been in. Roughly a third of these people clearly remembered seeing shelves full of books, even though no books were visible in the office (Brewer & Treyens, 1981). In this case, the participants had “supplemented” their memory of the office, relying on the common knowledge that professors’ offices usually do hold a lot of books. They then lost track of the source of this “sup-plement”—and so lost track of the fact that the books came from their own beliefs and not from this particular experience.
How does this pattern apply to
emotional memories? We have already said that emotional events tend to be
remembered vividly: Often, we feel like we can “relive” the distant event,
claiming that we recall the event “as though it were yesterday.” But as
compelling as they are, these memories—like any memories—are open to error. In
one study, researchers surveyed college students a few days after the tragic explosion
of the Space Shuttle Challenger in
1986. Where were the students when they heard about the explosion? Who brought
them the news? Three years later, the researchers questioned the same students
again. The students confidently reported that, of course, they clearly
remembered this horrible event. Many students reported that the memory was
still painful, and they could recall their sense of shock and sadness upon
hearing the news. Even so, their recollections of this event were, in many
cases, mis-taken. One student was certain she was sitting in her dorm room
watching TV when the news was announced; she recalled being deeply upset and
telephoning her par-ents. It turns out, though, that she had heard the news in
a religion class when some people walked into the classroom and started talking
about the explosion (Neisser & Harsh, 1992).
Similar results have been
recorded for many other events. For example, surveys of current college
students usually show that they vividly remember where they were when they
heard the news, of the September 11, 2001, attacks on the United States and how
they heard it. When we take steps to check on these memories, however, we often
find substantial errors. Many of the students are completely confident—but
mistaken— about how they learned of the attacks, who brought them the news,
what their initial responses were (e.g., P. Lee & Brown, 2004; Pezdek,
2004). Similar memory errors can be documented even in George W. Bush, U.S.
president at the time of the attacks (Figure P.10). Bush confidently recalled
where he was when he heard the news, and what he was doing at the time—but his
recollection turned out to be mistaken (D. Greenberg, 2004). Being president,
it seems, is no protection against memory errors.
What’s going on in all of these
cases? When we experience a trau-matic event—like the Challenger explosion, or the September 11 attacks—we’re likely to
focus on the core meaning of the event and pay less attention to our own
personal circumstances. This means that we will record into memory relatively
little information about those circumstances. Later, when we try to recall the
event, we’re forced to reconstruct the setting as best we can. This
reconstruction process is generally accurate, but certainly open to error. As a
result, our recollection of emotional events can be vivid, detailed,
compelling—and wrong.
Related Topics
Privacy Policy, Terms and Conditions, DMCA Policy and Compliant
Copyright © 2018-2023 BrainKart.com; All Rights Reserved. Developed by Therithal info, Chennai.