The Biological
Roots of Smiling
All animals interact with other
members of their species, whether as mates, parents, off-spring, or
competitors. And these interactions, in turn, usually depend on some kind of
communication as each animal lets the other know about its status and
intentions. Sometimes, the style of communications is species specific—pertaining to just one species—but often the
communication involves signals shared by many types of animals. Thus, many
mammals use the same “surrender” display to end a fight: They lie on their
backs, exposing their bellies and their throats, as a way of communicating
(roughly) “I know I’ve lost the fight; I’m giving in, and making myself
completely vulnerable to you. Let’s not fight any more.”
Humans, too, have various ways of
communicating their status and intentions. In many circumstances, of course, we
use language—and so we can, with enormous preci-sion, convey our message to a
conversational partner. We also communicate a great deal by our body
position—how close we stand to another person, how we hold our arms, how we
orient our bodies, and so on. Our faces convey still more information—
including the various facial displays that express emotion.
Virtually all babies start
smiling at a very young age. The first smiles are detectable in infants just 1
month old; smiles directed toward other people are evident a month or so later.
One might think that babies learn to
smile by observing others and imitating the facial expressions they see, but
evidence argues against this proposal. For example, babies who are born blind
start smiling at the same age as sighted babies, and—just like sighted
babies—they’re most likely to smile when interacting with others and when
they’re com-fortable and well fed. Likewise, one study compared the facial
expressions of three groups of athletes receiving their award medals at the
2004 Paralympic Games (Matsumoto & Willingham, 2009). One group had been
blind since birth; a second group had some years of visual experience but was
now fully blind; a third group had normal sight. The study showed essentially
no difference among these groups in their facial expressions.
Apparently, then, the behavior of
smiling is something that humans do
without a history of observational learning. On this basis, we might expect to
find smiles in all humans in all cultures—and we do. In other words, the
behavior of smiling is species general—observable
in virtually all members of our species. In one study, American actors were
photographed while conveying emotions such as happiness, sadness, anger, and
fear. These photos were then shown to members of an isolated tribe in New
Guinea, and individuals there were asked to pick the emotion label that matched
each photograph. Then the procedure was reversed: The New Guinea tribesmen were
asked to portray the facial expression appropriate to specific situations, such
as happiness at the return of a friend or anger at the start of a fight.
Photographs of their performances were shown to American college students, who
were asked to judge which situation the tribesmen had been trying to convey in
each photo (Ekman & Friesen, 1975).
Performance in these tasks was quite good—the New Guinea tribesmen were generally able to recognize the Americans’ expressions, and vice versa. Importantly, performance varied from one emotion to the next, with (for example) disgust and fear more difficult to recognize than sadness. The expression producing the most accurate identification, though, was happiness. Smiles are apparently a universal human signal (Ekman, 1994; Izard, 1994).
The behavior of smiling is not
just shared across cultures; it is also shared across species. In other words, we find similar emotional expressions in
animals with genomes simi-lar to ours. Thus, smiling is species general (found in the entire species), but it is not species specific (found only in one
species).
Darwin himself was especially
interested in this point and drew evidence from several sources, including his
own observations of animals at the London Zoo. He eventually published his
findings in a book entitled The
Expression of the Emotions inMan and Animals (Darwin, 1872). Darwin’s
observations made it clear to him that therewere different types of smiles—a
point that has been well confirmed in more recent research. The smiles can be
differentiated in terms of the situations that elicit them; they can also be
differentiated by their exact appearance. And, remarkably, the various types of
smiles can be identified as readily in other species as they can in humans.
One type of smile seems
straightforwardly expressive of your
inner state, and it’s pro-duced when you feel happy. This smile will be
produced even if no other people are around, and it involves both a change in
mouth shape (the corners of the lips are pulled upward) and a shift in the
muscles of the upper face, surrounding the eyes. The latter shift creates the
pattern often called crow’s feet—lines
that radiate outward from the eyes (Figure 2.16).
The expressive smile obviously
occurs in humans when events or stimuli please us, or when we hear a good joke.
A similar expression occurs in young monkeys in the midst of play; many
observers interpret it as a signal between monkeys that their pushing and
tumbling is playful rather than aggressive. Smiles in humans also promote
cooperation: A smile on someone’s face often signals their intention to
cooperate and, at the same time, the sight of a smile tends to evoke positive
feelings in the perceiver, making cooperation more likely.
A different sort of smile seems
more polite in nature, and it’s rarely
produced without an audience. In this smile, one pulls the corners of one’s
lips upward but with little movement of the eyes. This sort of smile seems to
function as a greeting and also as a means of defusing situations that
otherwise might be tense or embarrassing (Goldenthal, Johnston, & Kraut,
1981). It’s also a smile people use when they wish to simulate happiness (e.g.,
when pretending to have fun at a dreadful party, or when trying to persuade
someone they’re amusing when they are actually boring).
This polite smile can also be
found in nonhuman primates, where it generally takes a form of drawing back the
lips and revealing the teeth, but keeping the teeth plainly closed. In monkeys,
this smile may be a gesture of submission at the end of a conflict, or it may
be intended to avoid a conflict. It’s
as if one monkey is saying to another, “Look—my teeth are closed; I’m obviously
not preparing to bite you or fight you. So be good to me” (Figure 2.17).
What does all of this imply about
the origins of smiling? The fact that
smiles emerge with no history of learning (e.g., in individuals blind since
birth) tells us that this behavior is strongly shaped by inborn (genetic)
factors. This claim is certainly consis-tent with the universality of smiling
(across cultures and across species), which sug-gests an ancient origin for
this behavior: It’s likely that the smiles evident in American college
students, in New Guinea tribesmen, or in playful monkeys were shaped by natural
selection long ago, in the ancestors shared by all of these modern primates.
Moreover, the obvious function of smiles in modern creatures supplies an
important
clue as to why the behavior of smiling served our ancestors well—by providing
a signal about emotions and intentions that facilitated social interactions.
These observations in turn provide a clear indication of why this behavior was
promoted and preserved by natural selection. All of these are key points
whenever we try to establish—whether we’re focusing on smiles or any other
trait—how and why the trait evolved.
Related Topics
Privacy Policy, Terms and Conditions, DMCA Policy and Compliant
Copyright © 2018-2023 BrainKart.com; All Rights Reserved. Developed by Therithal info, Chennai.