Home | | Psychology | Making Observations: Systematically Collecting Data

Chapter: Psychology: Research Methods

Making Observations: Systematically Collecting Data

Suppose we’ve defined our question and figured out how to measure our dependent variable.

Systematically Collecting Data

Suppose we’ve defined our question and figured out how to measure our dependent variable. What comes next? In everyday life, people often try to answer empirical ques-tions by reflecting on their personal experience. They’ll try to recall cases that seem rel-evant to the issue being evaluated, and say things like, “What do you mean, boys are more aggressive? I can think of three girls, just off the top of my head, who are extremely aggressive.”

This sort of informal “data collection” may be good enough for some purposes, but it’s unacceptable for science. For one thing, the “evidence” in such cases is being drawn from memory—and people’s memories aren’t always accurate. Another problem is that memories are often selective. We’ll consider a pattern known as confirmation bias. This term refers to people’s tendency to recall evidence that confirms their views more easily than they can recall evidence to the contrary. So if someone happens to start out with the idea that girls are more aggres-sive, confirmation bias will lead them to supporting evidence—whether their initial idea was correct or not.

More broadly, scientists refuse to draw conclusions from anecdotal evidence— evidence that involves just one or two cases, has been informally collected, and is now informally reported. There are many reasons for this refusal: The anecdotes are often drawn from memory; and, for the reasons just described, that by itself is a problem. What’s more, anecdotes often report a conclusion drawn from someone’s observations, but not the observations themselves, and that invites questions about whether the con-clusion really does follow from the “evidence.” For example, if someone reports an anec-dote about an aggressive girl, we might wonder whether the girl’s behavior truly deserved that label—and our uncertainty obviously weakens the effect of the anecdote.

Perhaps the worst problem with anecdotal evidence, though, is that the anecdotes may represent exceptions and not typical cases. This is because, when reporting anec-dotes, people are likely to overlook the typical cases precisely because they’re familiar and hence not very interesting. Instead, they’ll recall the cases that stand out either because they’re extreme or because they don’t follow the usual pattern. As a result, anec-dotal evidence is often lopsided and misleading.

If we really wanted to compare boys’ and girls’ aggression, then, how could we avoid all these problems? For a start, we need to record the data in some neutral and objective way, so there’s no risk of compromising the quality of our evidence through bias or memory errors. Second, our data collection has to be systematic (Figure 1.5). We might want to collect observations from all the boys and all the girls at a particular play-ground; or perhaps, just to keep our project manageable, we might observe every tenth child who comes to the playground. Notice that these will be firsthand observations— we’re not relying on questionable anecdotes. Notice also that our selection of which children to observe (“every tenth child”) is not guided by our hypothesis. That way, we can be sure we’re not just collecting cases that happen to be unusual, or cases that sup-port our claim. This sort of data collection can be cumbersome, but it will give us data that we can count on in drawing our conclusions.


Study Material, Lecturing Notes, Assignment, Reference, Wiki description explanation, brief detail
Psychology: Research Methods : Making Observations: Systematically Collecting Data |


Privacy Policy, Terms and Conditions, DMCA Policy and Compliant

Copyright © 2018-2023 BrainKart.com; All Rights Reserved. Developed by Therithal info, Chennai.