Search
and detect
Predators
can search for prey actively or passively. Water column searchers, such as
herrings, anchovies, minnows, tunas, and billfishes rely heavily on vision, as
do nocturnal plankton feeders. Olfaction, gustation, and hearing are also
important for some water column searchers, particularly sharks. Low-frequency
sounds of 20–300 Hz are especially attractive to sharks, whereas amino acids
elicit feeding responses in many predatory fishes. Smell, taste, touch, or
electrolocalization (passive or active) are employed extensively by benthic and
nocturnal foragers such as eels, catfishes, gymnotid knifefishes, sea robins
(triglids), goatfishes (mullids), and threadfins (polynemids), with polyodontid
paddlefishes apparently using electrical cues to find plankton swarms.
Chemoreception and touch are used by other groups that possess barbels, such as
sturgeons, minnows, cods, and croakers. Some fishes search by speculation, much
as chickens scratch where buried prey are likely to occur. Goatfishes move
along the bottom probing into sediments with their muscular barbels that are
equipped with abundant taste receptors; some goatfishes flush prey by inserting
their mobile barbels into refuge holes where prey have sought shelter (Hobson
1974). Boxfishes (Ostraciidae) and triggerfishes (Balistidae) expel jets of
water from their mouths to blast sand away from potential buried prey. Logperch
(Percidae) roll stones with their snouts in search of hidden insect larvae.
These speculating foragers frequently have attendant species that follow them
and snap up prey disturbed by
the
forager’s activity.
The
energy expended in active search can be saved by camouflaged predators that lie
in wait on the bottom or in other structure. Such camoufl age is often termed protective
resemblance when hiding from predators, or aggressive resemblance when
lying in wait (the latter usage is inaccurate behaviorally since “aggression”
should be reserved for combat situations between animals, not for predatory
activities). Benthic, camouflaged predators lie on rocks or soft bottoms or can
be slightly (or greatly) buried by sediment. Their skin is colored to resemble
algae-covered rocks, tunicates, sponges, and other bottom types. Wartlike and
other fleshy outgrowths of skin and fins are common. These fish rush
explosively from the bottom to capture prey or open their typically large
mouths rapidly and inhale prey. Many scorpionfishes (Scorpaenidae), flatheads
(Platycephalidae), seabasses (Serranidae), and hawkfishes (Cirrhitidae) rest
exposed on the bottom, whereas lizardfishes (Synodontidae), stonefishes
(synanceine scorpaenids), stargazers (Uranoscopidae), and flatfishes Pleuronectiformes)
lie with only their eyes exposed above the sediment. For such liein- wait
predators, vision is the primary sense mode by which prey are detected, except
for the elasmobranchs which may also use electrical cues. Many benthic,
immobile ambushers appear surprisingly conspicuous, at least to a human
observer. They may rely on prey habituating to their presence and thus
growing careless.
Some
water column predators, including countershaded or silvery-sided fishes such as
gars (Lepisosteidae), pikes (Esocidae), and barracuda (Sphyraenidae), also lie
in wait, floating motionless near or below the surface and darting at prey that
fail to recognize them. This group also includes substrate- and leaf-mimicking
species such as trumpetfishes (Aulostomidae) and leaffishes (Nandidae). Many
predators shift among search patterns. Trumpetfish lie in wait among gorgonian
corals to ambush roving prey, hide behind swimming herbivores such as
parrotfishes, or swim actively in the water column and attack relatively stationary
schools of zooplanktivores. By day, torpedo rays erupt from the sand at prey
that have wandered over them, whereas at night they swim actively above the
bottom in search of swimming prey. Prey behavior and density often determine
which search mode will be employed. For example, young lumpfish (Cyclopteridae)
cling to rocks with their modified pelvic fins and make short excursions to
feed on nearby zooplankton when prey densities are high. At low prey densities,
the larvae swim through the water column searching for and feeding on
plankters, thereby incurring the greater costs of active search but avoiding
starvation (Brown 1986; Helfman 1990).
Considerable
attention has been paid to the search tactics and detection capabilities of
zooplanktivorous fishes. Fish swim through the water column scanning an area
ahead of them that is shaped approximately like a hemisphere, the widest part
being closest to the fish. The volume of this search space, the distance from
objects at which fish react, and the size object that a fish is capable of
detecting change with fish size, water clarity, illumination level, and current
speed. Large juveniles can detect smaller objects than can small juveniles, and
most fishes react further away in clearer water or after light levels exceed
some threshold value (Hairston et al. 1982). Zooplanktivores that feed in
currents employ searching tactics that vary as a function of current speed.
Fish remain in place and wait for food objects to approach them; upon
detection, fish then swim toward prey at low current velocities (10–14 cm/s)
but fall back with the current at higher speeds (McFarland & Levin 2002).
Reaction
distance is
heavily dependent on prey size, to the extent that most zooplanktivores will
react to and pursue the largest appearing prey in their visual field. This
means that a small zooplankter near a fish may be taken preferentially to a
larger plankter farther away because the smaller prey appears larger (the apparent
size hypothesis). However, prey immobility and location also affect
selection, smaller prey being preferred if they are mobile or are more directly
in front of the forager (O’Brien et al. 1985; O’Brien 1987). The speeds at
which fish search appear to approach the optimal in terms of maximizing intake
relative to energy expense. For example, the actual sustained search speed of a
40 cm salmonid is 3 body lengths per second (BL/s), which is close to the
calculated optimum sustained speed of 2.9 BL/s (Ware 1978; Hart 1993). Speeds
vary as a function of fish size (=metabolic rate) and food concentration.
Although
group formation is most commonly viewed as an antipredator response, grouped
fishes may search more successfully than individuals. Foragers in groups may
locate food sooner, ingest food faster, have more time available for foraging,
and grow faster than solitary foragers. For example, in minnows (Phoxinus
phoxinus, Cyprinidae), Goldfish (Carassius auratus, Cyprinidae), and
Stone Loaches (Noemacheilus barbatulus, Cobitidae), shoal members spend
less time before finding food than do solitary individuals, and the benefit
increases with increasing shoal size (Pitcher & Parrish 1993). Accelerated
rates arise because a fish in a shoal can search for food while simultaneously
watching for signs of successful feeding in shoal mates, thus increasing the
area over which it effectively searches. Also, the time each individual spends
scanning for predators may decrease, leaving more time for feeding. These
benefits are countered by intragroup competition for food, competition
increasing as the group size increases.
Related Topics
Privacy Policy, Terms and Conditions, DMCA Policy and Compliant
Copyright © 2018-2023 BrainKart.com; All Rights Reserved. Developed by Therithal info, Chennai.