Applications
in Forensic Psychiatry
The most
serious problem involves possible effects of hyp-nosis on memory. There is
evidence that hypnosis can distort memory in two ways: through confabulation, the creation of
pseudomemories that are reported as real, or through concret-ing, an unwarranted increase in the confidence with which
hyp-notized individuals report their memories, either true or false (McConkey,
1992; Orne et al., 1985; Spiegel and
Spiegel, 1986; Spiegel and Vermutten, 1994). Hypnosis can facilitate the recall
of dissociated memories, especially when recall is hampered by the strong
affect associated with trauma (Kardiner and Spiegel, 1947; Spiegel, 1984,
1992). However, the research literature in-dicates that the most clearly
reproducible problem is the produc-tion of confident errors, exaggerating the
true value of memories unearthed in hypnosis.
This is
especially a problem when the memories involve witnessing a crime and civil or
criminal action in court is a pos-sibility. Most states prohibit the use of
hypnotically induced testimony, but some prescribe witnesses who have been
hypno-tized regarding the content of their potential testimony (Spiegel and
Scheflin, 1994). If the possibility exists that a patient may be required to
testify, it is wise to discuss the use of hypnosis with the patient and (with
the patient’s permission) the patient’s attorney or the district attorney and
obtain written agreement re-garding its use. If the patient is likely to be
called to testify, the therapist should obtain electronic recording of all contact
with the patient (preferably on videotape) so the court can examine for
possible suggestive influences. Full guidelines for use of hypno-sis in the
forensic setting include careful debriefing of the subject before hypnosis is
employed, the use of nonleading questions, complete videotaping of all contact
with the patient, and careful debriefing afterward (Spiegel and Spiegel, 1986).
The
controversies surrounding so-called false memory syndrome have reignited
questions regarding the validity of the material recovered by the use of
hypnosis. One of the most com-mon applications of hypnosis in the court and
legal settings had been refreshing recollection of witnesses and victims of
crimes. Even though the current controversy focuses on the dangers of hypnotically
induced confabulation or excessive confidence in memories (Diamond, 1980),
there have been some positive results. A well-known example is the case
involving the driver of a hijacked school bus in Chowchilla, California (People v. Schoenfeld, 1980). Under hypnosis, the bus driver was able to recall the license plate number of the
car driven by the kidnap-pers. This information, not consciously available to
him before hypnotic intervention, led to the arrest and conviction of the
criminals.
Because
hypnosis involves a suspension of critical judg-ment, and therefore a state of
heightened suggestibility or re-sponsivity to social cues, it is important that
the interview be conducted with a minimum of inserted information. To minimize
the risk of contaminating subjects’ memories, it is important to use open-ended
questions, such as, “What happens next?” rather than, “How did he sexually
abuse you?”
Information
retrieved with the aide of hypnosis may simply be the result of an additional
recall trial (Erdelyi and Kleinbard, 1978), it may be new and true (Dywan and
Bowers, 1983), or it may be a confabulation (Laurence and Perry, 1983). It may
indeed be a combination of all three (Orne et
al., 1985). As a result, courts have long been unwilling to admit the
testimony of a person hypnotized while testifying and have also begun to
exclude testimony of witnesses who have previously been hypno-tized about the
event in question. Even when a subject is acting in good faith, hypnosis can
amplify both truth and falsehood. A good guideline is that hypnosis increases
the recovery of memo-ries, both true and confabulated.
On the
other hand, people do dissociate during trauma, often failing to recall events
despite being conscious at the time (Cardeña and Spiegel, 1993; Spiegel and
Cardeña, 1991). There is evidence that such memory gaps may persist for years
or even decades after such traumatic events as physical or sexual abuse
(Williams, 1994).
Therapists
treating victims of sexual abuse must be aware that the use of hypnosis may
compromise the ability of a witness to testify in court. After much legal
battling, some courts now allow witnesses to testify after the use of hypnosis
provided that certain guidelines are followed (California Leg-islature, 1985).
These relate primarily to the training and inde-pendence of the professional
doing the hypnotic interrogation and the electronic recording of the entire
process (Spiegel and Spiegel, 1986).
To
address this controversial issue, the Council on Sci-entific Affairs of the
American Medical Association convened a panel of experts to examine the
research evidence relevant to this problem. The report issued by the panel
concluded that the existing evidence indicates that the use of hypnosis tends
to increase the productivity of witnesses, resulting in new memories, some of
which are true and some of which are incor-rect (Orne et al., 1985). The panel recommended that careful guidelines
similar to the ones adopted by the state of Califor-nia be followed when
hypnosis is used in the forensic setting (Table 72.9).
As a rule, it is advisable to caution attorneys and wit-nesses that the use of hypnosis might open the possibility of challenge to the credibility or even the admissibility of a wit-ness. The kind of situation in which hypnosis is most likely to be worth the risk is one in which there is a traumatic amnesia for the events of a crime or in which all other avenues of exploration have been exhausted. Hypnosis is by no means a truth serum, and the courts must weigh the effects of any hypnotic procedure on a witness.
The
medico–legal aspects of the use of hypnosis in psychiatric practice have been
discussed at length elsewhere (Maldonado and Spiegel, 2002a, 2002b).
Most
outcome studies, of which there have been a number, have resulted in two main
conclusions related to the therapeutic uses of hypnosis. First, there is no
doubt that hypnosis is effective. Secondly, the degree of hypnotizability is
predictive of treatment response (Spiegel et
al., 1981b). It also has been observed that hypnosis is particularly useful
and yields better results when it is specifically requested by the patient
(Glick, 1970; Lazarus, 1973).
Related Topics
Privacy Policy, Terms and Conditions, DMCA Policy and Compliant
Copyright © 2018-2023 BrainKart.com; All Rights Reserved. Developed by Therithal info, Chennai.