Approaches to Classification
Historically, there have been two fundamental
approaches to formulating systems of psychiatric classification: etiological
and descriptive. Etiology-based classification systems organize categories
around pathogenetic processes so that disorders cor-responding to a particular
category share the same underlying cause. Because the etiological basis for
most psychiatric condi-tions remains elusive, etiological classification
systems tend to be based instead on a particular conceptualization of the
process of mental disorders. Although such classifications may be
heuris-tically useful to proponents of the particular conceptualization that
forms the basis of the system, they are often considerably less useful for
proponents of different etiological principles, which greatly limits their
utility. For this reason, a descriptive approach to classification has proved
to be of greater utility given our current understanding. The descriptive
approach aims to es-chew particular etiological theories and instead relies on clinical
descriptions of presenting symptoms. This approach, advanced by the work of the
19th century psychiatrist Emil Kraepelin, formed the basis for the system of
classification of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders,
Third Edition (DSM-III) introduced in
1980. As a result, DSM-III and its successors, the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Third Edition, Revised (DSM-III-R) and
DSM-IV, have proved to be useful in a variety of different settings and by
psychiatrists of widely different backgrounds and conceptual orientations.
Given that the manual lacks a specific etiological conceptualiza-tion, what is its organizing principle? The fundamental element is the syndrome, that is, a group or pattern of symptoms that appear together temporally in many individuals. It is assumed that these symptoms cluster together because they are associated in some clinically meaningful way, which perhaps may reflect a common etiological process, course, or treatment response Although it was hoped that the syndromes identified in the DSM represented relatively homogeneous subpopulations of patients, over the past 20 years since the publication of these definitions in DSM-III, the goal of discovering common etiolo-gies for each of the DSM-defined syndromes has remained elu-sive. Epidemiologic and clinical studies have shown extremely high rates of comorbidities among the disorders, undermining the hypothesis that the syndromes represent distinct etiologies. Furthermore, epidemiologic studies have shown a high degree of short-term diagnostic instability for many disorders. With regard to treatment, lack of specificity in treatment response is the rule rather than the exception. The efficacy of many psycho-tropic medications cuts across the DSM-defined categories. For example, the selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) have been demonstrated to be efficacious in a wide variety of disor-ders from many different sections of the DSM, including major depressive disorder, panic disorder, obsessive–compulsive dis-order, dysthymic disorder, bulimia nervosa, social anxiety disor-der, post traumatic stress disorder generalized anxiety disorder, hypochondriasis, body dysmorphic disorder and borderline per-sonality disorder. Results of twin studies have also contradicted the DSM assumption that separate syndromes have a different underlying genetic basis. For example, twin studies have shown that generalized anxiety disorder and major depressive disorder may share the same genetic risk factors (Kendler, 1996), and evidence from molecular genetics research (Berrettini, 2000) indicates that three of the putative susceptibility loci associated with DSM-defined bipolar disorder also contribute to the risk of DSM-defined schizophrenia.
Given these clear limitations in the syndromal
approach, it is important that users of the DSM resist the temptation to reify
the DSM diagnostic categories as if they were actual dis-eases. They are best
viewed as clinically useful constructs that are helpful in facilitating
communication and record keeping and in selecting treatment. As more
information about the causes of mental disorders become evident over the next
decades, it is more than likely that the syndromal approach will be replaced by
a classification system that is more reflective of the underlying eti-ology and
pathophysiology.
The diagnoses included in DSM-IV are defined
categorically, that is, diagnostic criteria are provided that indicate whether
an individual’s clinical presentation either meets or does not meet the
diagnostic criteria for a particular disorder. This method of classification is
similar to what is used in other fields in medicine, namely that a patient
either has or does not have a particular diagnosis, like pneumonia, colon
cancer, multiple sclerosis, and so on. This tendency to define illness in terms
of categories is undoubtedly due to the fact that it is reflective of basic
human thought processes, embodied by the use of nouns in everyday speech to
indicate categories of “things’’ (e.g., chairs, tables, dogs, cats).
In principle, however, variation in the
symptomatology can be represented by a set of dimensions rather than by
mul-tiple categories. An example of this in medicine is blood pres-sure, which
is measured along a continuum from low to high. (It only becomes categorical
when we apply the label “hyperten-sion’’ to indicate that a patient has a
significant elevated level of blood pressure that puts him or her at risk for
developing serious illness.) While a categorical approach to classification has
important heuristic appeal, it may not represent the true state of things.
Implicit in the categorical approach is an as-sumption that mental disorders
are discrete entities, separated from one another and from normality, either by
recognizably distinct combinations of symptoms or by demonstrably dis-tinct
etiologies. While this has been shown to be the case for a small number of
conditions (e.g., Down syndrome, fragile X syndrome, phenylketonuria,
Alzheimer’s disease, Huntington’s disease and Creutzfeldt–Jakob disease), there
is little evidence supporting the applicability of this model for most other
psy-chiatric symptoms. Indeed, in the last 20 years, the categori-cal approach
has been increasingly questioned as evidence has accumulated that the so-called
categorical disorders like major depressive disorder and anxiety disorders, and
schizophrenia and bipolar disorder seem to merge imperceptibly both into one another
and into normality (Kendler and Gardner, 1998) with no demonstrable natural
boundaries.
Dimensional approaches do have some clear
advan-tages. First of all, the commonly observed phenomena of ex-cessive
comorbidity (i.e., an individual receiving multiple, simultaneous DSM
diagnoses) is arguably a direct result of having a categorical system with more
than 250 categories. A dimensional approach, which would characterize an
indi-vidual’s psychopathology by indicating the extent of his or her psychiatric
symptomatology across a number of dimensions, virtually eliminates apparent
comorbidity. For example, con-sider an individual who presents with depression,
anxiety and social avoidance. Using the DSM-IV categorical system, cri-teria
might be met for three diagnoses (i.e., major depressive disorder, social
phobia and generalized anxiety disorder), thus warranting a diagnosis of all
three disorders on Axis I. A di-mensional approach may simply indicate that the
person has “high scores’’ on the depression, anxiety and social avoidance
dimensions. Another advantage of the dimensional approach is that it avoids
setting particular thresholds for distinguishing between pathology and
normality. Rather than categorically saying that an individual has major depressive
disorder, a di-mensional approach might say that the person is high on the
depression dimension.
There are a number of practical problems that
potentially limit the utility of adopting a dimensional approach. First of all,
clinicians are accustomed to thinking in terms of diagnostic categories, and
the existing knowledge base about the presen-tation, etiology, epidemiology,
course, prognosis and treatment is based on these categories. Furthermore,
decisions about the management of individual patients (e.g., whether to treat
and with what type of treatment) are also much easier to make if the patient is
thought of as having a particular disorder (with its associated prognostic and
treatment implications) rather than as a profile of scores across a series of
dimensions. It should be noted that, at least for personality disorders,
current dis-satisfaction with the categorical approach has led to proposals for
research that might allow for the adoption of a dimensional approach to
classifying personality functioning in DSM-V (First et al., 2002).
Related Topics
Privacy Policy, Terms and Conditions, DMCA Policy and Compliant
Copyright © 2018-2026 BrainKart.com; All Rights Reserved. Developed by Therithal info, Chennai.