Propositions and Hypotheses
Figure shows how theoretical constructs such
as intelligence, effort, academic achievement, and earning potential are
related to each other in a nomological network. Each of these relationships is
called a proposition. In seeking explanations to a given phenomenon or
behavior, it is not adequate just to identify key concepts and constructs
underlying the target phenomenon or behavior. We must also identify and state
patterns of relationships between these constructs. Such patterns of
relationships are called propositions. A proposition is a
tentative and conjectural relationship between constructs that is stated in a
declarative form. An example of a proposition is: 'An increase in student
intelligence causes an increase in their academic achievement.' This
declarative statement does not have to be true, but must be empirically
testable using data, so that we can judge whether it is true or false.
Propositions are generally derived based on logic (deduction) or empirical
observations (induction).
Because propositions are associations between
abstract constructs, they cannot be tested directly. Instead, they are tested
indirectly by examining the relationship between corresponding measures
(variables) of those constructs. The empirical formulation of propositions,
stated as relationships between variables, is called hypotheses (see
Figure 2.1). Since IQ scores and grade point average are operational measures
of intelligence and academic achievement respectively, the above proposition
can be specified in form of the hypothesis: 'An increase in students' IQ score
causes an increase in their grade point average.' Propositions are specified in
the theoretical plane, while hypotheses are specified in the empirical plane.
Hence, hypotheses are empirically testable using observed data, and may be
rejected if not supported by empirical observations. Of course, the goal of hypothesis
testing is to infer whether the corresponding proposition is valid.
Hypotheses can be strong or weak. 'Students'
IQ scores are related to their academic achievement' is an example of a weak
hypothesis, since it indicates neither the directionality of the hypothesis
(i.e., whether the relationship is positive or negative), nor its causality
(i.e., whether intelligence causes academic achievement or academic achievement
causes intelligence). A stronger hypothesis is 'students' IQ scores are positively related
to their academic achievement', which indicates the directionality but not the
causality. A still better hypothesis is 'students' IQ scores have positive
effects on their academic achievement', which specifies both the directionality
and the causality (i.e., intelligence causes academic achievement, and not the
reverse). The signs in Figure 2.2 indicate the directionality of the respective
hypotheses.
Also note that scientific hypotheses should
clearly specify independent and dependent variables. In the hypothesis,
'students' IQ scores have positive effects on their academic achievement,' it
is clear that intelligence is the independent variable (the 'cause') and
academic achievement is the dependent variable (the 'effect'). Further, it is
also clear that this hypothesis can be evaluated as either true (if higher
intelligence leads to higher academic achievement) or false (if higher
intelligence has no effect on or leads to lower academic achievement). Later on
in this book, we will examine how to empirically test such cause-effect
relationships. Statements such as 'students are generally intelligent' or 'all
students can achieve academic success' are not scientific hypotheses because
they do not specify independent and dependent variables, nor do they specify a
directional relationship that can be evaluated as true or false.
Related Topics
Privacy Policy, Terms and Conditions, DMCA Policy and Compliant
Copyright © 2018-2023 BrainKart.com; All Rights Reserved. Developed by Therithal info, Chennai.